GET THE APP

ASD-relevant Animal Models of the Foxp Family of Transcription Fa
Autism-Open Access

Autism-Open Access
Open Access

ISSN: 2165-7890

+44 1223 790975

Review Article - (2012) Volume 0, Issue 0

ASD-relevant Animal Models of the Foxp Family of Transcription Factors

J. Michael Bowers and Genevieve Konopka*
Department of Neuroscience, The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, Dallas, TX 75390-9111, USA
*Corresponding Author: Genevieve Konopka, Department of Neuroscience, The University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, 5323 Harry Hines Blvd., ND4.300, Dallas, TX 75390-9111, USA, Tel: 214-648-5135 Email:

Abstract

Autism is a neurodevelopmental disorder with a multifaceted association between genes and the environment. Currently, in the majority of patients, the etiology of autism is not known and coupled with increasing prevalence rates, along with the high degree of heritability of autism, the development of animal models is crucial for studying and developing therapies for autism. A key characteristic of autism is marked abnormalities in the acquisition and use of language. Thus, to understand and ultimately treat autism is an especially difficult task because no animal produces language, as it is defined in humans. In this review, we will discuss the FOXP family of genes, which are a group of transcription factors that have been linked to both autism, as well as language in humans. Due to the association of language/communication and the Foxp family of transcription factors, animal models with targeted disruptions of Foxp functioning are powerful tools for understanding the developmental signaling pathways that may be vulnerable in autism.

Keywords: Foxp2; Foxp1; Autism; Genetics; Rodent vocalization

Introduction

The etiology of autism, or the more broadly defined Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD), is multifaceted and is likely due to a combination of genetic and environmental interactions [1,2]. Genetically modified animal models are amenable for studying these interactions, and for testing biochemical therapeutics in a high-throughput manner. Nevertheless, it is imperative for any proposed animal model of autism to recapitulate the basic fundamental core features of the disorder, as those observed in humans and for these “symptoms” to be reversed by the same drugs used to treat patients [3]. This definition of a valid animal model is complicated in the development of ASD animal models because disrupted communication is a core feature of ASD, and language is perceived to be a human-specific feature. Thus, here we present animal models for genes implicated in language, Foxp2 and Foxp1, and discuss their relevancy to understanding the pathophysiology of ASD.

FOXP Family

The FOXP family of transcription factors consists of three genes expressed in the brain: Foxp1, Foxp2, and FOXP4. Of these three genes, more is known about Foxp2 due to the identification of a mutation in Foxp2 over a decade ago, in a large intergenerational family termed the KE family [4]. Affected members of the KE family were found to have a mutation in the DNA-binding domain of Foxp2, which resulted in these individuals exhibiting verbal dyspraxia, a below average IQ, and syntactic impairments in language [5,6]. Subsequent studies have shown that this heterozygous mutation of an arginine to histadine (R553H) in the forkhead DNA binding domain of Foxp2, prevents the Foxp2 protein from binding to DNA [7], and thus likely inhibits its ability to directly regulate transcriptional events. Additional types of mutations including a premature stop mutation, leading to protein truncation have been identified in other individuals and families with similar speech and language phenotypes (for a detailed review of these mutations [8,9]). Foxp2 has a highly conserved amino acid sequence, as well as a conserved distribution in brain regions that are involved in vocal communication, such as the neocortex, basal ganglia and cerebellum [8]. This conservation across species in areas of the brain implicated in diseases such as ASD [10], makes the study of Foxp2 animal models relevant to understanding the normal neurodevelopmental mechanisms of these brain regions and circuits. In humans, Foxp2 has had accelerated divergence from a common ancestor sequence with chimpanzees, as evidenced by two humanspecific amino acids in its sequence [11]. This rapid change in the molecular evolution of Foxp2 occurred around the same time as the emergence of language in the human lineage, and these changes have also been shown to be important for a unique transcriptional program for the human form of Foxp2 [12]. Foxp2 transcriptionally regulates genes involved in neuronal development, neurite outgrowth, dendritic branching, and axonal morphology [12-14]. Therefore, the evidence for Foxp2 involvement in language, its expression pattern in the brain, and its transcriptional regulation of important neurodevelopmental genes has led to the hypothesis that Foxp2 may have a role in neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD.

Several studies have investigated whether genetic variations in Foxp2 itself are associated with ASD; however, since there have been both positive and negative results reported in the literature, these studies are inconclusive [15-23]. In addition, recent genome-wide surveys of Copy Number Variation (CNV) in ASD samples have not found variation in Foxp2 associated with the disorder [24-26]. Nevertheless, strong support for Foxp2 playing a role in ASD can be found in its regulation of downstream signaling pathways. For example, candidate gene approaches have found that Foxp2 regulates the expression of CNTNAP2, MET, and SRPX2, all ASD related genes [27-29]. In addition, genome-wide DNA binding and gene expression studies have identified numerous target genes of Foxp2 in either human or mouse tissue that are associated with ASD [14,30-32]. Together, these studies suggest that signaling pathways downstream of Foxp2 regulation of gene expression are particularly vulnerable in ASD.

Foxp1 is highly homologous to Foxp2, and is also expressed in the developing human brain, in both neocortex and basal ganglia [33]. For a more detailed review on expression patterns of FOXP family members, see [32,34]. The conservation of functional domains and overlapping expression in certain brain regions among FOXP family members suggests the potential for functional redundancy, and/or synergistic activities. In fact, Foxp1, Foxp2, and FOXP4 not only form homodimers, but they also have the capacity to form heterodimers with each other, to regulate transcription [35]. Thus, through this heterodimerization mechanism, both Foxp1 and FOXP4 may participate in Foxp2-mediated signaling pathways, important for speech and language. Unlike Foxp2, a more direct association between Foxp1 and ASD has been discovered recently. Several studies have uncovered mutations, deletions, or copy number variations of Foxp1 in individuals with ASD, or other neurodevelopmental disorders such as intellectual disability [36-42]. In addition, some affected individuals with Foxp1 mutations have abnormally enlarged ventricles, as assessed by MRI [37]. The discovery of increased brain volume in patients with a Foxp1 mutation is consistent with previous reports of ASD patients [43-45]. Similar to Foxp2, Foxp1 can also repress expression of CNTNAP2, suggesting convergent signaling pathways of ASD genes by FOXP family members [39]. Moreover, these results again highlight a potential mechanism for Foxp2 participation in signaling pathways vulnerable in ASD, through its heterodimerization with Foxp1. Additional studies of Foxp1 targets will be required to fully assess the involvement of Foxp1-regulated signaling pathways in ASD. Taken together, these data support a role for both Foxp1 and Foxp2 in the regulation of signaling pathways involved in ASD.

Rodent Ultrasonic Vocalizations

The most challenging aspect of ASD biology to model in rodent systems is the disruption to language and communication. However, it has been well established that rodents communicate through the use of Ultrasonic Vocalizations (USVs) [46,47], and numerous studies have identified alterations in USVs in mice, with genetic modifications in genes associated with ASD [48-52]. USVs are produced by many species of rodents in the context of social encounters, and function as a short-distance communication method since USVs attenuate rapidly in space [53,54]. Moreover, many predators have limited hearing capacity for ultrasonic tones, thus providing rodents an ideal method for communicating with intended receivers, while minimizing the threat of predation [46,54]. USVs emerge early in infancy in many rodent species, (i.e. within the first few days of life for mice and rats) [47,55]. Pup distress USVs are triggered by aversive events, such as maternal separation and hypothermia [56,57]. These maternal separation calls are the most widely used method for eliciting and analyzing vocalizations in young rodents [58-60]. Maternal separation calls are effective stimuli because these infant vocalizations readily elicit maternal retrieval behavior in many strains of rats and mice [61-64]. The most parsimonious interpretation of these data is that pups’ ultrasonic calls communicate an aversive emotional state to their mothers, resulting in pup retrieval. Changes in USVs may thus, alter the behavioral feedback circuit between pup and mother leading to potential changes in other behavioral responses during development, such as anxiety-related behaviors [65,66]. Pups cease producing separation-induced distress calls around the time when their eyes and ear canals open at approximately 14 days of age [67], further suggesting that these particular vocalizations are not intended for non-maternal conspecifics [55]. The study of pup USVs, within the context of ASD is therefore, not only important for understanding mechanisms of vocalizations, but also how alterations in biobehavior feedback at an early age may impact later development.

Animal Models and the Foxp Family

Since disrupted communication ability is a core feature of ASD, and this particular facet of ASD is particularly challenging to address in animal models. The association between language/communication and the Foxp family of transcription factors makes animal models with disruption to Foxp functioning, potentially informative for understanding the molecular biology of this phenotype in ASD, as well as vocalizations in general. A number of Foxp2 mutant mice have been developed, and the study of these animals have provided insight into the contribution of Foxp2 to vocalizations and other behaviours, that may be relevant to the study of ASD, such as learning and memory (see table 1 for a complete summary of these lines).

Gene Reference Mutation USV Other behavior Anatomy
Foxp2 Shu (2005) Null Allele PN 6; PN 10    
    Foxp2-/- 1) Reduced total number
2) Duration, frequency, bandwidth comparable to WT
1) Severe motor defects
2) Die by PN 21
1) Atypical cerebellar growth
2) Reduced postnatal weight gain
    Foxp2 -/+ 1) Reduced total number duration, peak frequency, bandwidth comparable to WT 1) Similar to WT
on Morris water maze
1) Minor changes in cerebellum
2) Reduced postnatal weight gain
  F re n c h (2007)   Nu l l Allele cKO      
    Foxp2 -/-   1) Severe motor defects
2) Die by PN 21
  1) Atypical cerebellar growth
2) Reduced postnatal weight gain
    Foxp2 -/+   1) No gross motor defects 1) Normal cerebellar growth
2) Normal postnatal weight gain
  Fu jit a (2008) K E family mutation P N 8    
    Foxp2R552H/R552H 1) Reduced total number and produced mainly “clicks” 1) Severe motor defects
2) Die by PN 21
1) Atypical cerebellar growth
2) Reduced postnatal weight gain
    Foxp2 R552H/+ 1) Decrease in total number
2) Able to produce a variety
of calls but with reduced duration
1) Slight motor defects 1) Normal postnatal weight gain
  Groszer (2008) KE family mutation PN4    
    Foxp2 R552H/R552H 1) No reduction in total number 1) Severe motor defects
2) Die by PN 21
1) Atypical cerebellar growth
2) Reduced postnatal weight gain
    Foxp2 R552H/+ 1) No differences 1) No gross motor defects
2) Slight reduction in motor
skill learning
1) Impaired LTD in striatum
2) Climbing fiber and parallel fiber input on to Purkinje
cells were normal but faster induction of LTD
3) Normal postnatal weight gain
4) Normal cerebellar growth
  Enard (2009) Humanized Foxp2 PN 4, PN 7, PN 10, PN 13    
    Foxp2hum/hum 1) No differences in
total number of calls or duration
2) Lower start frequency and
lower min and max frequency
1) No gross motor defects
2) Reduced exploratory
activity in a novel environment
1) Reduction in dopamine brain that express Foxp2
2) Longer neurite outgrowth
3) Stronger LTD
4) Changes in gene expression patterns in the striatum
    Foxp2WT/KO   1) Slightly impaired motor
learning on the rotarod
1) Increase in dopamine concentrations in all regions of
brain that express Foxp2
2) Increases in serotonin levels in nucleus accumbens
3) Shorter neurite outgrowth
4) Gene expression showed opposite patterns than
observed in Foxp2hum/hum
  Gaub (2010);
French (2011);
Kurt (2012)
KE family mutation PN 4    
    Foxp2 R552H/R552H 1) No reduction in total number
2) More low acoustic USVs
with similar duration as WT
  1) Reduced postnatal weight gain
    Foxp2 R552H/+ 1) No reduction in total number
and similar duration as WT
1) Learned auditory-motor
association task slowly
but reached WT levels
1) Normal postnatal weight gain
2) Striatal neurons showed more negative modulation
than the characteristic positive modulation
3) Higher than normal ongoing firing rates for medium
spiny neurons
    Foxp2S321X/S321X 1) Small reduction in total
number
2) More low acoustic USVs
similar duration, and fewer
frequency jumps as WT
  1) Reduced postnatal weight gain
    Foxp2S321X/+ 1) No reduction in total
number
2) More high acoustic USVs
but longer duration than WT
1) Did not learn auditory motor
association task
1) Normal postnatal weight gain
  Rousso (2008) Null Allele      
    Foxp1-/-     1) Foxp1 involved in motor
neuron formation in spinal cord
  Surmeli (2011) Motor neuron cKO      
    Olig2: Cre;
Foxp1flox/flox
    1) Foxp1 involved in sensory-motor connectivity of
neurons in spinal cord
  Rousso (2012) Null Allele      
    Foxp4-/-     1) Increased neural tube defects, lack of lateral ventricles,
and holoprosencephaly

PN=postnatal day; WT= wild type; cKO=conditional knockout.

Table 1: Summary of Foxp family mouse models including data for USVs, other behaviors, and anatomy.

Shu et al. [68] generated the first Foxp2 Knockout (KO) mouse. Shu et al. [68] found that homozygous loss of Foxp2 (Foxp2-/-) in mice leads to severe motor defects, and the mice typically die by postnatal day 21. The subsequent generation of an additional line ofFoxp2-/- mice, using a conditional null allele approach has supported this finding [69]. In addition, theFoxp2-/- mice also have reduced postnatal weight gains, although the underlying etiology of this is unknown, and could be tied to non-nervous system requirements for Foxp2 function [68-70]. Moreover, heterozygous loss of Foxp2 (Foxp2-/+) also leads to a reduction in postnatal weight gain in one strain of mice [68], but unlike theFoxp2-/- mice, the Foxp2-/+ mice are viable [68,69]. In addition to the development of Foxp2 null allele mice, other Foxp2 mutants have been developed. Mice have been engineered with a specific mutation in Foxp2, analogous to the mutation in the affected members of the KE, mice family (R552H), or with a nonsense mutation in Foxp2 analogous to the mutation in an additional family with disruptions in speech and language (S321X) [9,70]. Finally, mice with a “humanized” form of Foxp2 have been generated by substituting the exon that yields the two human-unique amino acids of Foxp2 into the orthologous exon of the mouse Foxp2 gene [71]. The resultant Foxp2hum/hum mice are viable, and do not have any growth issues.

Anatomically, major developmental abnormalities in the cerebellum have been reported in both homozygous null and point mutation Foxp2 mutants. These mice were found to have atypical cerebellar growth, e.g. nonconforming alignment of Purkinje cells and incomplete migration of granular cells [68-70,72]. In addition, one report has found heterozygous null Foxp2 mutants to have slight differences in cerebellar development, as compared to wild type mice [68], while the other studies have not reported differences in heterozygotes [69,70]. Alterations in the cerebellum are relevant to the study of ASD, as there is increasing evidence that cerebellar deficits are associated with ASD [10]. Moreover, Foxp2R552H/+ mice were found to have impaired Longterm Depression (LTD) in the striatum, but a more rapid induction of LTD in the cerebellum [70]. These same mice were also tested using in vivo multielectrode recordings and displayed negative modulation of striatal neuron firing rates, which is in contrast to the positive modulation exhibited by wild type mice [73]. Additionally, these Foxp2R552H/+ mice also demonstrate abnormally high continuous striatal activity [73]. This pattern of activation in the striatum is irregular, because the majority of neurons in the striatum typically show low in vivo firing rates [74]. In contrast to the impaired LTD of Foxp2R552H/+ mice, Foxp2hum/hum mice have increased LTD in the striatum [71]. These changes in excitation/inhibition, and/or plasticity may also be relevant to many of the biological changes ascertained in patients with ASD [2].

In addition to anatomical abnormalities, homozygous Foxp2 mutants have discernible motor deficiencies as measured by righting reflex, negative geotaxis, and rotarod testing [68-70,72]. In contrast,Foxp2-/+ mutants, Foxp2R552H/+, and Foxp2hum/hum mice, either did not exhibit motor deficits, or displayed minor deficits in motor skill acquisition [68,70,72]. A recent study examining the Foxp2R552H/+, and Foxp2S321X/+ mice has found that these two different point mutations affect the ability of these mice to learn how to associate auditory stimuli with a motor output behavior [75]. In this study, two different acoustic tones were played. The mice were conditioned to jump a hurdle separating two chambers, in response to one tone, but abstain from jumping the hurdle, in association with a different tone. Foxp2R552H/+ mice learned the task, but at a slower rate than wild type mice, whereas Foxp2S321X/+ mice exhibited a slow, flat learning curve that never reached wild type levels [75]. These data suggest an important distinctive behavioral outcome, as a consequence of two different human-based mutations of the Foxp2 gene on learning behavior. These studies also highlight the involvement of Foxp2 in brain circuitry, underlying different types of learning that may be vulnerable in neurodevelopmental disorders, such as ASD.

Examination of USVs in Foxp2 mouse models has found that both heterozygous and homozygous Foxp2 mutant mice show differences in the total number of vocalizations emitted, compared to wild type litter mates [68,72]. In contrast, Groszer et al. [70] report finding no reduction in the total number of USVs emitted by Foxp2 mutants, compared to controls [76]. An analysis of the acoustic properties of the vocalizations produced by both homozygous and heterozygous Foxp2 mutants yielded no differences in the call duration, frequency, or bandwidth [68,70,72]. Interestingly, studies using humanized Foxp2 mice have found that the vocalizations of these mice have a lower frequency onset, and lower minimum and maximum frequency means [71]. An extension of previous work by Gaub et al. [76] found Foxp2R552H/R552H mice produced: 1) the same total number of USVs and 2) these USVs were the same duration as the USVs produced by wild type mice, with the only distinction being that the USVs were produced at a lower frequency. In contrast, Foxp2S321X/S321X mice produced fewer total numbers of USVs, and those USVs were produced at a lower frequency, with no significant difference in the duration of the calls, as compared to wild type mice. Moreover, Foxp2R552H/+ and Foxp2S321X/+ mice showed no difference in the total number and duration of the USVs, as compared to wild type mice, however, these two mutant lines tended to produce USVs, at a higher frequency than those calls produced by Foxp2R552H/R552H and Foxp2R552H/R552H mice [76]. Because much of the neural circuitry underlying USVs still remains unknown, it is unclear which aspects of Foxp2 function and expression are driving this altered behavior. Moreover, it is currently believed that mouse vocalizations are innate, and are not learned in a manner that is similar to language in humans, although there is some recent evidence that adult vocalizations in the male mouse may have features reminiscent of vocal learning [77]. Thus, it is plausible that maternal separation USVs only recapitulate motor aspects of language, and may not serve as appropriate models for the cognitive aspects of language. However, as discussed above, there are other social and communicative aspects of USVs that occur between pup and mother, making them an appropriate model for the study of autistic-like behaviors in rodents. In addition, the gene expression studies that have been carried out in both Foxp2 knockout mice [14] and Foxp2 humanized mice [71], have identified several ASD genes as potential Foxp2 targets in these animal models. Therefore, it is possible that evolutionary conserved Foxp2 targets are important for the coordination of motor movements responsible for both USVs and language.

Foxp2 is also expressed in zebra finch brain, in areas comparable to the mammalian expression pattern [33]. In addition, Foxp2 expression changes during song acquisition, pointing to an important role for Foxp2 in learned song, which is in contrast to its potential role in inherent USVs in mice [33,78-80]. Furthermore, reduction of Foxp2 in the zebra finch brain leads to a decrease in spine formation, as well as a disruption to song learning [13,81]. Changes in neurite outgrowth have also been found in mouse models of Foxp2, [14,71], thereby suggesting a conserved cellular function for Foxp2, that may have a functional impact on vocalization circuitry. Together, these data from rodent and songbird emphasize a critical role for Foxp2 in the motor circuitry underlying vocalizations. What these data do not address is the function of human Foxp2 in the cognitive aspects of language. However, gene expression studies comparing human and chimpanzee Foxp2 support the idea that human Foxp2 has a unique transcriptional program that may be contributing to higher cognition and language [12]. Further studies of Foxp2 animal models, such as conditional knockout of Foxp2 in specific brain regions will facilitate understanding the role of Foxp2 in brain development. Such experiments, together with gene expression studies in human and non-human primate cells and tissues, should assist in parsing out the conserved function of Foxp2 in vocalizations, as well as potential human-specific functions of Foxp2 in language signaling pathways.

Recent work in genetically modified rodents has also implicated Foxp1 in Central Nervous System (CNS) motor function. Homozygous Foxp1 knockout mice are embryonic lethal at E14.5 due to defects in the cardiovascular system [82], making the study of the role of Foxp1 in behavior impossible to ascertain in these mice. However, examination of the developing spinal cord in these mice has shown that Foxp1 is required for motor neuron specification [83,84]. In addition, a conditional knockout of Foxp1 in motor neurons leads to profound impairments in limb coordination during motor movements [85]. Additional mice with selective reduction of Foxp1 in other areas of the CNS should be informative as to a potential role for Foxp1 in USVs, social behavior, or repetitive behaviors. Furthermore, the targets of Foxp1 in the developing mammalian brain have yet to be determined, and should provide insight into whether Foxp1 also transcriptionally regulates signaling pathways containing ASD genes similar to Foxp2. Thus, animal models of both Foxp2 and Foxp1 that recapitulate key aspects of the behaviors affected in humans with ASD (i.e., vocalizations and complex social behaviors) have the potential for modeling key neurodevelopmental phenotypes, for which functional interventions and therapeutics can be tested.

Future Directions

Future studies utilizing whole genome sequencing in even larger patient cohorts than has been conducted for exome sequencing should more thoroughly address the contribution of both Foxp2 and Foxp1 in ASD. Since, there is a greater appreciation that the genetic architecture of ASD in most cases is due to polygenic contributions, rather than dominant single gene mutations [2], it will be even more important to understand the function and the gene targets of these transcription factors in the developing brain. In addition, the high homology, ability to heterodimerize, and overlapping expression patterns of FOXP4 to the other FOXP family members in the brain, make FOXP4 another prime candidate for study in animal models. However, since homozygous loss of Foxp4 is embryonic lethal due to cardiovascular and neural tube deficits [86,87], CNS-specific conditional alleles of Foxp4 will be warranted.

Since the development of reliable animal models is a critical step towards understanding and ultimately treating ASD, this task has been particularly daunting due to the inherently human-specific cognitive nature of language, that is characteristically disrupted in ASD. As such, the use of animal models for understanding ASD has some inherent limitations. However, the majority of signaling pathways and circuitry used in language most certainly has been built upon existing pathways, and circuitry utilized for vocalizations and working memory in other species. Thus, understanding these neurobiological features using animal models can provide important insights into developmental mechanisms at risk in ASD. In addition, animal models other than mice may provide novel insight. For example, rat pups have more complex vocalizations, including a phenomenon termed “maternal potentiation”, that may be useful in modeling ASD-like behaviors [65]. Since genetically modified rats (and potentially other organisms, such as non-human primates) can be rapidly generated using new technologies, such as zinc finger nucleases [88], both USVs and other cognitive behaviors may be better suited for study in other species, such as the rat. Due to the relationship of the FOXP family of transcription factors with vocalizations, language and cognitive diseases, these genes have the potential to bring unique insight into the pathophysiology of ASD. Although the uncovering of genes important for ASD and other neurodevelopmental disorders is still ongoing, the brain-expressed FOXP family members are rare examples of transcription factors important for brain development, language, and ASD.

Acknowledgements

Genevieve Konopka is a Jon Heighten Scholar in Autism Research. Genevieve Konopka and J. Michael Bowers are also supported by the NIMH (R00MH090238 and F32MH086258).

References

  1. Sheng L, Ding X, Ferguson M, McCallister M, Rhoades R, et al. (2010) Prenatal polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbon exposure leads to behavioral deficits and downregulation of receptor tyrosine kinase, MET. Toxicol Sci 118: 625-634.
  2. Berg JM, Geschwind DH (2012) Autism genetics: searching for specificity and convergence. Genome Biol 13: 247.
  3. McFarlane HG, Kusek GK, Yang M, Phoenix JL, Bolivar VJ, et al. (2008) Autism-like behavioral phenotypes in BTBR T+tf/J mice. Genes Brain Behav 7: 152-163.
  4. Lai CS, Fisher SE, Hurst JA, Vargha-Khadem F, Monaco AP (2001) A forkhead-domain gene is mutated in a severe speech and language disorder. Nature 413: 519-523.
  5. Vargha-Khadem F, Watkins K, Alcock K, Fletcher P, Passingham R (1995) Praxic and nonverbal cognitive deficits in a large family with a genetically transmitted speech and language disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 92: 930-933.
  6. Watkins KE, Dronkers NF, Vargha-Khadem F (2002) Behavioural analysis of an inherited speech and language disorder: comparison with acquired aphasia. Brain 125: 452-464.
  7. Vernes SC, Nicod J, Elahi FM, Coventry JA, Kenny N, et al. (2006) Functional genetic analysis of mutations implicated in a human speech and language disorder. Hum Mol Genet 15: 3154-3167.
  8. Fisher SE, Scharff C (2009) FOXP2 as a molecular window into speech and language. Trends Genet 25: 166-177.
  9. MacDermot KD, Bonora E, Sykes N, Coupe AM, Lai CS, et al. (2005) Identification of FOXP2 truncation as a novel cause of developmental speech and language deficits. Am J Hum Genet 76: 1074-1080.
  10. Amaral DG, Schumann CM, Nordahl CW (2008) Neuroanatomy of autism. Trends Neurosci 31: 137-145.
  11. Enard W, Przeworski M, Fisher SE, Lai CS, Wiebe V, et al. (2002) Molecular evolution of FOXP2, a gene involved in speech and language. Nature 418: 869-872.
  12. Konopka G, Bomar JM, Winden K, Coppola G, Jonsson ZO, et al. (2009) Human-specific transcriptional regulation of CNS development genes by FOXP2. Nature 462: 213-217.
  13. Schulz SB, Haesler S, Scharff C, Rochefort C (2010) Knockdown of FoxP2 alters spine density in Area X of the zebra finch. Genes Brain Behav 9: 732-740.
  14. Vernes SC, Oliver PL, Spiteri E, Lockstone HE, Puliyadi R, et al. (2011) Foxp2 regulates gene networks implicated in neurite outgrowth in the developing brain. PLoS Genet 7: e1002145.
  15. Gauthier J, Joober R, Mottron L, Laurent S, Fuchs M, et al. (2003) Mutation screening of FOXP2 in individuals diagnosed with autistic disorder. Am J Med Genet A 118A: 172-175.
  16. Newbury DF, Bonora E, Lamb JA, Fisher SE, Lai CSL, et al. (2002) FOXP2 Is Not a Major Susceptibility Gene for Autism or Specific Language Impairment. Am J Hum Genet 70: 1318-1327.
  17. Wassink TH, Piven J, Vieland VJ, Pietila J, Goedken RJ, et al. (2002) Evaluation of FOXP2 as an autism susceptibility gene. Am J Med Genet 114: 566-569.
  18. Gong X, Jia M, Ruan Y, Shuang M, Liu J, et al. (2004) Association between the FOXP2 gene and autistic disorder in Chinese population. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet 127B: 113-116.
  19. Li H, Yamagata T, Mori M, Momoi MY (2005) Absence of causative mutations and presence of autism-related allele in FOXP2 in Japanese autistic patients. Brain Dev 27: 207-210.
  20. Marui T, Koishi S, Funatogawa I, Yamamoto K, Matsumoto H, et al. (2005) No association of FOXP2 and PTPRZ1 on 7q31 with autism from the Japanese population. Neurosci Res 53: 91-94.
  21. Laroche F, Ramoz N, Leroy S, Fortin C, Rousselot-Paillet B, et al. (2008) Polymorphisms of coding trinucleotide repeats of homeogenes in neurodevelopmental psychiatric disorders. Psychiatr Genet 18: 295-301.
  22. Feuk L, Kalervo A, Lipsanen-Nyman M, Skaug J, Nakabayashi K, et al. (2006) Absence of a paternally inherited FOXP2 gene in developmental verbal dyspraxia. Am J Hum Genet 79: 965-972.
  23. Lin PI, Chien YL, Wu YY, Chen CH, Gau SS, et al. (2012) The WNT2 gene polymorphism associated with speech delay inherent to autism. Res Dev Disabil 33: 1533-1540.
  24. Sanders SJ, Murtha MT, Gupta AR, Murdoch JD, Raubeson MJ, et al. (2012) De novo mutations revealed by whole-exome sequencing are strongly associated with autism. Nature 485: 237-241.
  25. O'Roak BJ, Vives L, Girirajan S, Karakoc E, Krumm N, et al. (2012) Sporadic autism exomes reveal a highly interconnected protein network of de novo mutations. Nature 485: 246-250.
  26. Neale BM, Kou Y, Liu L, Ma'ayan A, Samocha KE, et al. (2012) Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders. Nature 485: 242-245.
  27. Mukamel Z, Konopka G, Wexler E, Osborn GE, Dong H, et al. (2011) Regulation of MET by FOXP2, genes implicated in higher cognitive dysfunction and autism risk. J Neurosci 31: 11437-11442.
  28. Vernes SC, Newbury DF, Abrahams BS, Winchester L, Nicod J, et al. (2008) A functional genetic link between distinct developmental language disorders. N Engl J Med 359: 2337-2345.
  29. Roll P, Vernes SC, Bruneau N, Cillario J, Ponsole-Lenfant M, et al. (2010) Molecular networks implicated in speech-related disorders: FOXP2 regulates the SRPX2/uPAR complex. Hum Mol Genet 19: 4848-4860.
  30. Spiteri E, Konopka G, Coppola G, Bomar J, Oldham M, et al. (2007) Identification of the transcriptional targets of FOXP2, a gene linked to speech and language, in developing human brain. Am J Hum Genet 81: 1144-1157.
  31. Vernes SC, Spiteri E, Nicod J, Groszer M, Taylor JM, et al. (2007) High-throughput analysis of promoter occupancy reveals direct neural targets of FOXP2, a gene mutated in speech and language disorders. Am J Hum Genet 81: 1232-1250.
  32. Bowers JM, Konopka G (2012) The role of the FOXP family of transcription factors in ASD. Dis Markers.
  33. Teramitsu I, Kudo LC, London SE, Geschwind DH, White SA (2004) Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 expression in songbird and human brain predicts functional interaction. J Neurosci 24: 3152-3163.
  34. Vargha-Khadem F, Gadian DG, Copp A, Mishkin M (2005) FOXP2 and the neuroanatomy of speech and language. Nat Rev Neurosci 6: 131-138.
  35. Li S, Weidenfeld J, Morrisey EE (2004) Transcriptional and DNA binding activity of the Foxp1/2/4 family is modulated by heterotypic and homotypic protein interactions. Mol Cell Biol 24: 809-822.
  36. Hamdan FF, Daoud H, Rochefort D, Piton A, Gauthier J, et al. (2010) De novo mutations in FOXP1 in cases with intellectual disability, autism, and language impairment. Am J Hum Genet 87: 671-678.
  37. Pariani MJ, Spencer A, Graham JM Jr, Rimoin DL (2009) A 785kb deletion of 3p14.1p13, including the FOXP1 gene, associated with speech delay, contractures, hypertonia and blepharophimosis. Eur J Med Genet 52: 123-127.
  38. Horn D, Kapeller J, Rivera-Brugués N, Moog U, Lorenz-Depiereux B, et al. (2010) Identification of FOXP1 deletions in three unrelated patients with mental retardation and significant speech and language deficits. Hum Mutat 31: E1851-E1860.
  39. O'Roak BJ, Deriziotis P, Lee C, Vives L, Schwartz JJ, et al. (2011) Exome sequencing in sporadic autism spectrum disorders identifies severe de novo mutations. Nat Genet 43: 585-589.
  40. Carr CW, Moreno-De-Luca D, Parker C, Zimmerman HH, Ledbetter N, et al. (2010) Chiari I malformation, delayed gross motor skills, severe speech delay, and epileptiform discharges in a child with FOXP1 haploinsufficiency. Eur J Hum Genet 18: 1216-1220.
  41. Talkowski ME, Rosenfeld JA, Blumenthal I, Pillalamarri V, Chiang C, et al. (2012) Sequencing chromosomal abnormalities reveals neurodevelopmental loci that confer risk across diagnostic boundaries. Cell 149: 525-537.
  42. Girirajan S, Brkanac Z, Coe BP, Baker C, Vives L, et al. (2011) Relative burden of large CNVs on a range of neurodevelopmental phenotypes. PLoS Genet 7: e1002334.
  43. Piven J, Arndt S, Bailey J, Havercamp S, Andreasen NC, et al. (1995) An MRI study of brain size in autism. Am J Psychiatry 152: 1145-1149.
  44. Hardan AY, Minshew NJ, Mallikarjuhn M, Keshavan MS (2001) Brain volume in autism. J Child Neurol 16: 421-424.
  45. Hazlett HC, Poe M, Gerig G, Smith RG, Provenzale J, et al. (2005) Arch Gen Psychiatry 62: 1366-1376.
  46. Nyby J, Whitney G (1978) Ultrasonic communication of adult myomorph rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2: 1-14.
  47. Sales G, Pye D (1974) Ultrasonic communication by animals, Chapman and Hall, London, UK.
  48. Tsai PT, Hull C, Chu Y, Greene-Colozzi E, Sadowski AR, et al. (2012) Autistic-like behaviour and cerebellar dysfunction in Purkinje cell Tsc1 mutant mice. Nature 488: 647-651.
  49. Schmeisser MJ, Ey E, Wegener S, Bockmann J, Stempel AV, et al. (2012) Autistic-like behaviours and hyperactivity in mice lacking ProSAP1/Shank2. Nature 486: 256-260.
  50. Won H, Lee HR, Gee HY, Mah W, Kim JI, et al. (2012) Autistic-like social behaviour in Shank2-mutant mice improved by restoring NMDA receptor function. Nature 486: 261-265.
  51. Peñagarikano O, Abrahams BS, Herman EI, Winden KD, Gdalyahu A, et al. (2011) Absence of CNTNAP2 leads to epilepsy, neuronal migration abnormalities, and core autism-related deficits. Cell 147: 235-246.
  52. Bangash MA, Park JM, Melnikova T, Wang D, Jeon SK, et al. (2011) Enhanced polyubiquitination of Shank3 and NMDA receptor in a mouse model of autism. Cell 145: 758-772.
  53. Whishaw IQ, Metz GA, Kolb B, Pellis SM (2001) Accelerated nervous system development contributes to behavioral efficiency in the laboratory mouse: a behavioral review and theoretical proposal. Dev Psychobiol 39: 151-170.
  54. Costantini F, D'Amato FR (2006) Ultrasonic vocalizations in mice and rats: social contexts and functions. Acta Zoologica Sinica 52: 619-633.
  55. Noirot E (1968) Ultrasounds in young rodents: II. Changes in age with albino rats. Anim Behav 16: 129-134.
  56. Blumberg MS, Sokoloff G (2001) Do infant rats cry? Psychol Rev 108: 83-95.
  57. Hofer MA (1996) Multiple regulators of ultrasonic vocalization in the infant rat. Psychoneuroendocrinology 21: 203-217.
  58. Branchi I, Santucci D, Alleva E (2001) Ultrasonic vocalisation emitted by infant rodents: a tool for assessment of neurobehavioural development. Behav Brain Res 125: 49-56.
  59. Ehret G (2005) Infant rodent ultrasounds -- a gate to the understanding of sound communication. Behav Genet 35: 19-29.
  60. Brudzynski SM, Kehoe P, Callahan M (1999) Sonographic structure of isolation-induced ultrasonic calls of rat pups. Dev Psychobiol 34: 195-204.
  61. Brunelli SA, Shair HN, Hofer MA (1994) Hypothermic vocalizations of rat pups (Rattus norvegicus) elicit and direct maternal search behavior. J Comp Psychol 108: 298-303.
  62. Cohen-Salmon C, Carlier M, Roubertoux P, Jouhaneau J, Semal C, et al. (1985) Differences in patterns of pup care in mice. V--Pup ultrasonic emissions and pup care behavior. Physiol Behav 35: 167-174.
  63. Hahn ME, Lavooy MJ (2005) A review of the methods of studies on infant ultrasound production and maternal retrieval in small rodents. Behav Genet 35: 31-52.
  64. White NR, Adox R, Reddy A, Barfield RJ (1992) Regulation of rat maternal behavior by broadband pup vocalizations. Behav Neural Biol 58: 131-137.
  65. Scattoni ML, Crawley J, Ricceri L (2009) Ultrasonic vocalizations: a tool for behavioural phenotyping of mouse models of neurodevelopmental disorders. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33: 508-515.
  66. Dichter GS, Brunelli SA, Hofer MA (1996) Elevated plus-maze behavior in adult offspring of selectively bred rats. Physiol Behav 60: 299-304.
  67. Hofer MA, Shair HN, Masmela JR, Brunelli SA (2001).Dev Psychobiol 39: 231-246.
  68. Shu W, Cho JY, Jiang Y, Zhang M, Weisz D, et al. (2005) Altered ultrasonic vocalization in mice with a disruption in the Foxp2 gene. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 102: 9643-9648.
  69. French CA, Groszer M, Preece C, Coupe AM, Rajewsky K, et al. (2007) Generation of mice with a conditional Foxp2 null allele. Genesis 45: 440-446.
  70. Groszer M, Keays DA, Deacon RM, de Bono JP, Prasad-Mulcare S, et al. (2008) Impaired synaptic plasticity and motor learning in mice with a point mutation implicated in human speech deficits. Curr Biol 18: 354-362.
  71. Enard W, Gehre S, Hammerschmidt K, Hölter SM, Blass T, et al. (2009) A humanized version of Foxp2 affects cortico-basal ganglia circuits in mice. Cell 137: 961-971.
  72. Fujita E, Tanabe Y, Shiota A, Ueda M, Suwa K, et al. (2008) Ultrasonic vocalization impairment of Foxp2 (R552H) knockin mice related to speech-language disorder and abnormality of Purkinje cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 105: 3117-3122.
  73. French CA, Jin X, Campbell TG, Gerfen E, Groszer M, et al. (2012) An aetiological Foxp2 mutation causes aberrant striatal activity and alters plasticity during skill learning. Mol Psychiatry 17: 1077-1085.
  74. Kreitzer AC, Malenka RC (2008) Striatal plasticity and basal ganglia circuit function. Neuron 60: 543-554.
  75. Kurt S, Fisher SE, Ehret G (2012) Foxp2 mutations impair auditory-motor association learning. PLoS One 7: e33130.
  76. Gaub S, Groszer M, Fisher SE, Ehret G (2010) The structure of innate vocalizations in Foxp2-deficient mouse pups. Genes Brain Behav 9: 390-401.
  77. Arriaga G, Zhou EP, Jarvis ED (2012) Of mice, birds, and men: the mouse ultrasonic song system has some features similar to humans and song-learning birds. PLoS One 7: e46610.
  78. et al. (2004) FoxP2 expression in avian vocal learners and non-learners. J Neurosci 24: 3164-3175.
  79. Teramitsu I, White SA (2006) FoxP2 regulation during undirected singing in adult songbirds. J Neurosci 26: 7390-7394.
  80. Miller JE, Spiteri E, Condro MC, Dosumu-Johnson RT, Geschwind DH, et al. (2008) Birdsong decreases protein levels of FoxP2, a molecule required for human speech. J Neurophysiol 100: 2015-2025.
  81. Haesler S, Rochefort C, Georgi B, Licznerski P, Osten P, et al. (2007) Incomplete and inaccurate vocal imitation after knockdown of FoxP2 in songbird basal ganglia nucleus Area X. PLoS Biol 5: e321.
  82. Wang B, Weidenfeld J, Lu MM, Maika S, Kuziel WA, et al. (2004) Foxp1 regulates cardiac outflow tract, endocardial cushion morphogenesis and myocyte proliferation and maturation. Development 131: 4477-4487.
  83. Dasen JS, De Camilli A, Wang B, Tucker PW, Jessell TM (2008) Hox repertoires for motor neuron diversity and connectivity gated by a single accessory factor, FoxP1. Cell 134: 304-316.
  84. Rousso DL, Gaber ZB, Wellik D, Morrisey EE, Novitch BG (2008) Coordinated actions of the forkhead protein Foxp1 and Hox proteins in the columnar organization of spinal motor neurons. Neuron 59: 226-240.
  85. Sürmeli G, Akay T, Ippolito GC, Tucker PW, Jessell TM (2011) Patterns of spinal sensory-motor connectivity prescribed by a dorsoventral positional template. Cell 147: 653-665.
  86. Li S, Zhou D, Lu MM, Morrisey EE (2004) Advanced cardiac morphogenesis does not require heart tube fusion. Science 305: 1619-1622.
  87. Rousso DL, Pearson CA, Gaber ZB, Miquelajauregui A, Li S, et al. (2012) Foxp-mediated suppression of N-cadherin regulates neuroepithelial character and progenitor maintenance in the CNS. Neuron 74: 314-330.
  88. Geurts AM, Cost GJ, Freyvert Y, Zeitler B, Miller JC, et al. (2009) Knockout rats via embryo microinjection of zinc-finger nucleases. Science 325: 433.
Citation: Michael Bowers J, Konopka G (2012) ASD-relevant Animal Models of the Foxp Family of Transcription Factors. Autism S1:010.

Copyright: © 2012 Michael Bowers J, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top