GET THE APP

Stress Response to Surgery, Anesthetics Role and Impact on Cognit
Anesthesia & Clinical Research

Anesthesia & Clinical Research
Open Access

ISSN: 2155-6148

+44 1223 790975

Review Article - (2015) Volume 6, Issue 7

Stress Response to Surgery, Anesthetics Role and Impact on Cognition

Aceto Paola1*, Lai Carlo2, Dello Russo Cinzia3, Perilli Valter1, Navarra Pierluigi3 and Sollazzi Liliana1
1Department of Anesthesiology and Intensive Care, Agostino Gemelli Hospital, Rome, Italy
2Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, University of Rome “Sapienza”, Rome, Italy
3Department of Pharmacology, Catholic University of Sacred Heart, Rome, Italy
*Corresponding Author: Aceto Paola, Department of Anaesthesiology and Intensive Care, Catholic University of Sacred Heart, Largo A. Gemelli, 8-00168, Rome, Italy, Tel: +39-06-30154507, Fax: +39-06-3013450 Email: ,

Abstract

The stress response to surgery includes a number of hormonal changes initiated by neuronal activation of the Hypothalamo-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis. Surgery is one of the most potent activators of ACTH and cortisol secretion, and increased plasma concentrations of both hormones can be measured few minutes after the start of surgery. A variable inhibition of ACTH-stimulated production of cortisol by anesthetic drugs has been demonstrated in clinical studies. Endocrine response to stress also includes a release of pituitary hormone prolactin which is poorly affected by anesthesia. These two hormones seem to mostly contribute to memory consolidation processes during anesthesia. This review summarizes the main aspects of interaction between stress response to surgery and cognition during general anesthesia.

Introduction

In recent years, many studies have shown an incomplete abolition of the cognitive faculties -such as memory and learning- during anesthesia despite the dull of consciousness [1-6]. It has been suggested that memory seems to occur more frequently during surgical stimulation regardless of anesthesia depth [7]. The stimulating effect of surgery may be explained by the fact that stress hormones released in response to pain target the hippocampus, prefrontal cortex and amidgala, brain areas involved in the complex memory circuits [8]. The consolidation process of long-term memory mainly takes place in the hippocampus and neocortex [9]. During physiological sleep, functional status of hippocampal-neocortical circuits could be modulated, according to recent hypotheses, by cortisol [10]. High levels of cortisol can disrupt the normal communication between the hippocampus and the neocortex impairing the process of memory consolidation [10]. On the contrary, the decreased levels of cortisol, which occur during slow wave sleep (SWS), are considered critical for memory consolidation. Other neuroendocrine mediators of stress, including prolactin (PRL), could be involved in modulating memory functions [11,12]. PRL increases the expression of corticotrophin-releasing hormone (CRH) which has been shown to enhance learning through hippocampal CRH-R1 regardless of its indirect attenuation of stress-induced hypothalamo-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis activity [13,14], probably mediated by modulation of neural pathways to hypothalamic paraventricular nuclei [15]. A variable inhibition of ACTH-stimulated production of cortisol by anesthetic drugs has been demonstrated in clinical studies [16,17]. On the other hand, a marked increase in PRL concentrations were observed independently of the anesthetic procedure [18,19].

The aim of this review was to examine the complex interaction between stress response to surgery, anesthetics and cognition by analyzing the current available literature on this issue.

Sympatho-adrenal response to surgery

The stress response to surgery includes a number of hormonal changes initiated by neuronal activation of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis [18]. Hypothalamic activation of the sympathetic autonomic nervous system results in an increased secretion of catecholamines from the adrenal medulla and in a release of norepinephrine from presynaptic nerve terminals. The increased sympathetic activity causes the well-recognized cardiovascular effects of tachycardia and hypertension. In addition, the function of certain visceral organs, including the liver, pancreas and kidney, is modified directly by efferent sympathetic stimulation and/or circulating catecholamines [18]. Anterior pituitary hormone secretion is stimulated by hypothalamic releasing factors [20]. The pituitary synthesizes corticotrophin or adrenocorticotropic hormone (ACTH) as part of a larger precursor molecule, pro-opiomelanocortin. The precursor is metabolized within the pituitary gland into ACTH, β-endorphin and an N-terminal precursor. The amount of growth hormone and PRL secreted from the pituitary gland is also secreted in response to a surgical stimulus. On the contrary, concentrations of the other anterior pituitary hormones, thyroid-stimulating hormone (TSH), follicle- 24 stimulating hormone (FSH) and luteinizing hormone (LH) do not change markedly during surgery. The posterior pituitary produces arginine vasopressin which has a major role as an antidiuretic hormone. It also has an endocrine function, as it stimulates secretion of pro-opiomelanocortin from the anterior pituitary gland in conjunction with CRH.

The stress response to surgery causes an increased production of hormones with catabolic function. The value of this self-healing mechanismis doubtful in the surgical context [18].

Cortisol: cortisol secretion is increased as a result of adrenal cortical stimulation by means of ACTH. A greater ACTH and cortisol plasma concentrations may be detected few minutes after the start of the surgery and its increase is related to the intensity of surgical stimulus [21]. Other than its known metabolic effects, cortisol exerts anti-inflammatory activity by reducing inflammatory mediators’ production [18]. The physiological feedback mechanism that leads to ACTH synthesis inhibition due to increased cortisol concentration is disrupted during surgery whereas the cortisol secretion may be reduced by anesthetic drugs.

PRL: PRL is a protein hormone of 199 amino acids with a structure similar to that of growth hormone. Secretion of PRL is increased as part of the stress response to surgery. It has a little metabolic activity. The physiological effects of increased secretion of PRL during surgery are unknown. However, PRL is supposed to regulate T-lymphocyte proliferation [22].

Activation of the stress response: The endocrine response is activated by afferent neuronal impulses from the site of injury. These impulses travel along sensory nerve roots through the dorsal root of the spinal cord, up the spinal cord to the medulla in order to activate the hypothalamus. The idea that local substances might influence some of the changes associated with surgery was advanced after the cytokines discovery. The cytokines have local effects of mediating and maintaining the inflammatory response to tissue injury, and are also implicated in some of systemic changes. After major surgery, the main released cytokines are interleukin-1 (IL-1), tumor necrosis factor-α (TNF-α) and IL-6. The initial reaction is the release of IL-1 and TNF-α from activated macrophages and monocytes in the damaged tissues. This process stimulates production and release of further cytokines, in particular, IL-6, the main cytokine responsible for inducing the systemic changes known as acute phase response [23]. The cytokines increase the cortisol secretion by stimulating ACTH release as a result of surgical stimulus but cortisol response to surgery suppresses cytokine production due to a negative feedback system [24]. It has been demonstrated that IL-6 secretion, but not other stress hormones production, may be reduced using less invasive surgical techniques (e.g. laparoscopic approach) [25].

Effect of general anesthesia on the stress response to surgery

Anesthesia exerts a variable action on hypothalamic, pituitary and adrenal hormonal secretion although it has little effect on the cytokine response to surgery because it cannot influence tissue trauma [26]. Blood levels of ACTH, cortisol, epinephrine, norepinephrine and PRL has been used for evaluating stress or nociception level in surgery [18].Most of drugs used, including neuroleptic drugs, opioids, thiopentone, propofol and sevoflurane have been found to stimulate PRL release during anesthesia [18,19,27]. In patients undergoing cardiac surgery, morphine and other opioids (fentanyl, sufentanil and alfentanil) abolished ACTH and cortisol secretion at clinically used doses before the start of cardiopulmonary bypass (CPB) [18]. After CBP start, opioids were unable to completely suppress stress response [18]. In patients undergoing pelvic surgery, fentanyl suppressed ACTH and cortisol secretion when administered before surgical incision but not when given after the start of surgery [28]. Complete inhibition of the stress response in patients undergoing open cholecystectomy using high doses of fentanyl resulted in severe postoperative respiratory depression [29]. Therefore, opioids seem to suppress cortisol production only at high dose. Etomidate suppresses corticosteroids production at the adrenal cortex by reversible inhibition of the enzyme 11-β-hydroxylase and the cholesterol side chain cleavage enzyme. An induction dose (0.3 mg kg-1) blocks the synthesis of aldosterone and cortisol for up to 8 h [30]. Benzodiazepines (midazolam 0.2-0.4 mg 30 kg-1 and infusion of 0.9-0.125 mg kg-1 h-1) may also inhibit steroid production at the hypothalamic-pituitary level, but the significance of this effect has not been established. Effects of propofol on the synpathoadrenal system are well documented [16,31]. A single induction dose of propofol can suppress cortisol but it do not block cortisol and aldosterone secretion in response to surgical stress [31,32]. Continuous infusion of propofol, at deep anesthesia doses, completely abolished circulating cortisol secretion during surgery [31]. As the ACTH response to surgery during anesthesia maintenance was similar with propofol or inhaled anesthetics, the inhibition of cortisol secretion likely occur on the adrenal glands [16]. In the clinical setting of laparoscopic surgery, also the type of volatile anesthetic significantly affects the stress response: sevoflurane significantly decreased plasma concentrations of ACTH, cortisol and GH when compared to isoflurane [17].

Finally, there are evidences that perioperative use of dexmedetomidine reduces serum cortisol levels although the decrease in cortisol levels was not statistically different from the comparator anesthetics [33].

Interaction between stress hormones and cognition

Hormones released in response to stress play crucial roles in cognition. The influence of stress hormones on hippocampal function has been acknowledged for several decades. Stress is best described as a disturbance of physiological and psychological homeostasis ultimately controlled by HPA axis activity and resulting in secretion of corticosteroids from the adrenal cortex. The hippocampus has the highest concentration of corticosterone receptors in the brain that is responsible for the profound effects of stress on learning and memory processes [34]. Identification of the mechanisms by which stress modulate hippocampal function has been the subject of intense interest. A predictable finding is that high levels of glucocorticoids have a profound inhibitory effect on hippocampal cells activity, whereas low levels of glucocorticoids enhance cellular activity. Therefore, high concentrations of circulating glucocorticoids, consistent with marked stress, inhibit long-term potentation (LTP) while low concentrations of glucocortocoids enhanced LTP [35]. Within the central nervous system (CNS), two kinds of receptors are activated by cortisol: so-called glucocorticoid receptors (GRs; type II), and mineralocorticoid receptors (MRs; type I). When a neuron contains receptors of both types, as many within the hippocampus do, cortisol level affects the hippocampal function in an inverted U-shaped fashion [36]. For example, Diamond et al. [37] demonstrated that glucocorticoids facilitate LTP at low levels but impair it at high levels. The mechanisms producing this complex interaction are interesting. Type I (MR) receptors have a considerably higher affinity for cortisol than type II (GR) receptors do. Until all the MRs are occupied, there is a little occupancy of GRs. With extensive activation of the MR receptors, the possibility of activation of GRs emerges. Thus, impairment of hippocampal function by high levels of cortisol depends on the co-localization of MRs and GRs. Some study has confirmed that MRs and GRs are co-localized in the dentate gyrus and in the CA1 field, but much less so in the CA2 and CA3 fields, where concentrations of GR are greatly diminished [38,39]. This difference has potentially critical functional implications. Most prominently, it means that at high levels of cortisol, communication between the hippocampus and the neocortex, which is mediated by CA1 → subiculum → rhinal cortex connectivity, will be altered or disrupted. At the same time, communication within parts of the hippocampus itself, most prominently CA3, could remain intact. Thus, as the night progresses and cortisol levels increase, hippocampal-neocortical communication will eventually be altered. Note that levels between 10 and 30 µg/dL are associated with memory impairment during wake [40,41]. However, neither cortical-cortical activity itself nor activity within the CA3 circuits of the hippocampal formation will necessarily be disrupted. This interruption of hippocampal-neocortical communication could halt the consolidation of some type of memory, e.g. episodic memory [42,43], but it do not affect neuronal CA3 circuits activity or consolidation within procedural memory circuits [44]. High levels of cortisol during late night REM sleep could do more than interfere with episodic memory consolidation [45], as it weaken hippocampal system communication with the neocortex that is critical for episodic memory [46]. Neocortical circuits can thus generate only “episode-like” fragments that can be rather bizarre. It is worth noting the similarities between the nature of dreams and the kind of memories created during stress or trauma [47]. Fragmentation is an important feature of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), in which patients sometimes describe gaps in recalled experiences, not only of trauma but of other personal experiences as well [48,49]. There are clinical evidences that high levels of cortisol alter memory function. Patient populations with chronically elevated levels of cortisol, such as Cushing’s syndrome, major depression, and schizophrenia, as well as asthmatic patients treated with the glucocorticoid prednisone are characterized by impaired memory function [50,51]. Experimental studies of acute stress and memory have been carried out in animals [52], and several well-controlled studies have recently been conducted in humans. Kirschbaum et al. [40] demonstrated that a single, low dose of hydrocortisone (10 mg) leads to a deficit in verbal episodic memory. In this study, subjects who received hydrocortisone recalled fewer words (via a cued recall test) from a previously learned word list than control subjects did when recall interview occurred 60 min after receiving the drug. Further, Lupien et al. [53] demonstrated that prolonged cortisol elevations in older adults are associated with reduced hippocampal volume and impairments in hippocampally dependent memory tasks. Exactly how stress or glucocorticoids suppress LTP is still not well understood. It has been proposed that stress/glucocorticoids induce a variety of effects, including a change of the after-hyperpolarization amplitude, calcium current, or glutamate transmission, which impair LTP activation [54]. However, each stress hormone seems to affect LTP in a different manner. It has been demonstrated that noradrenaline and CRH increase LTP [55]. PRL, that pass the blood-brain barrier [56,57] and is also synthesized in the brain [57-60], acts as neuromodulator of HPA axis activation indirectly affecting LTP. PRL activates the mitogen activated protein kinase/extracellular signal-regulated kinase (MAPK/ERK) pathway facilitating CRH transcription in CRH neurons, This suggest that the in vivo reported inhibitory effect of PRL on HPA axis activity is indirect and probably mediated through modulation of afferent pathways to the PVN. PRL thus enhances CRH expression which has been shown to enhance learning through hippocampal CRFR1 [11].

There is only one study in which the relationship between memory and stress hormones under anesthesia has been investigated [61]. From this study it emerges that listening to a recording during surgical anesthesia is associated with a lower PRL increase, compared to baseline values, indicating that the perception of auditory stimulus during general anesthesia may affect neurohormonal response. In this study, hormonal changes were related to dream recall with a cortisol increase -compared to baseline values- determining a lower probability to recall dreams after awakening and higher levels of PRL leading to an increased rate of dream recall [61]. The interaction between a decrease in PRL levels induced by auditory presentation and memory impairment should be taken into consideration in order to ensure postoperative amnesia [61].

It is well known that there are no direct methods to measure stress level during general anaesthesia [62]. The common measured signs of autonomic reactions, such as blood pressure or heart rate, have been used for assessing stress level during anaesthesia, accepting their low specificity [62]. Some electroencephalographic (EEG-) derived variables, such as entropy [63] and bispectral index [64], are useful for monitoring depth of anaesthesia and preventing conscious recall but they do not always indicate inadequate analgesia and should be interpreted carefully during anaesthesia. The so-called surgical pleth index (SPI), a novel multivariate index using two continuous derived cardiovascular variables, has been proposed as a method to evaluate intraoperative stress level during general anaesthesia and a moderate correlation to the stress hormones (ACTH, cortisol, epinephrine, and norepinephrine) has been found during general anaesthesia in a recent study [65].

The incomplete abolition of memory and learning during anaesthesia, due to unimpaired stress response, may lead to an awareness episode with consequent PTSD, which is the most dangerous result of light anaesthesia [66]. HPA axis hyper-responsiveness in response to acute stress seems also involved in postoperative delirium pathophysiology [67].

As previously stated, the other aspect of stress response induced by maior surgery is inflammatory process that has a main role in the development of postoperative cognitive dysfunction (POCD) [68]. It has been demonstrated that surgery causes a persistent and possibly irreversible decrement in memory and learning in a murine model of Alzheimer disease, primarily through a transient activation of neuroinflammation [69] However, also metabolic endocrine stress, measured as perioperative cortisol secretion, has be found to correlate with incidence of POCD after major surgery [70]. Neuro-monitoring may help to prevent both PTSD [66] and POCD [71,72], but response to stress induced by surgery needs to be abolished in order to prevent memory consolidation and neuro-inflammation processes.

Conclusions

Anaesthetic drugs exert a variable action on response of HPA axis to surgical trauma and only little effects on cytokine production linked to tissue trauma. It is essential to blunt the stress hormones secretion in order to prevent postoperative complications, such as PTSD, POCD and delirium. Up to date, there is not a safe method to discriminate if response to stress is suppressed under anaesthesia; however, the use of drug at dose known to produce adequate anaesthesia and the support of neuro-monitoring may help to prevent an excessive HPA axis activation.

References

  1. Ghoneim MM, Block RI (1997) Learning and memory during general anesthesia: an update. Anesthesiology 87: 387-410.
  2. Schwender D, Kaiser A, Klasing S, Peter K, Pöppel E (1994) Midlatency auditory evoked potentials and explicit and implicit memory in patients undergoing cardiac surgery. Anesthesiology 80: 493-501.
  3. Aceto P, Valente A, Gorgoglione M, Adducci E, De Cosmo G (2003) Relationship between awareness and middle latency auditory evoked responses during surgical anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 90: 630-635.
  4. Merikle PM, Daneman M (1996) Memory for unconsciously perceived events: evidence from anesthetized patients. Conscious Cogn 5: 525-541.
  5. Aceto P, Congedo E, Lai C, Valente A, Gualtieri E, et al. (2007) Dreams recall and auditory evoked potentials during propofolanaesthesia. Neuroreport 18: 823-826.
  6. Aceto P, Perilli V, Lai C, Sacco T, Modesti C, et al. (2014) Minimum alveolar concentration threshold of sevoflurane for postoperative dream recall. Minerva Anestesiol .
  7. Deeprose C, Andrade J, Varma S, Edwards N (2004) Unconscious learning during surgery with propofolanaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 92: 171-177.
  8. Cahill L, McGaugh JL (1998) Mechanisms of emotional arousal and lasting declarative memory. Trends Neurosci 21: 294-299.
  9. Payne JD, Nadel L (2004) Sleep, dreams, and memory consolidation: the role of the stress hormone cortisol. Learn Mem 11: 671-678.
  10. Radulovic J, Rühmann A, Liepold T, Spiess J (1999) Modulation of learning and anxiety by corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) and stress: differential roles of CRF receptors 1 and 2. J Neurosci 19: 5016-5025.
  11. Croiset G, Nijsen MJ, Kamphuis PJ (2000) Role of corticotropin-releasing factor, vasopressin and the autonomic nervous system in learning and memory. Eur J Pharmacol 405: 225-234.
  12. Donner N, Bredewold R, Maloumby R, Neumann ID (2007) Chronic intracerebral prolactin attenuates neuronal stress circuitries in virgin rats. Eur J Neurosci 25: 1804-1814.
  13. Torner L, Toschi N, Pohlinger A, Landgraf R, Neumann ID (2001) Anxiolytic and anti-stress effects of brain prolactin: improved efficacy of antisense targeting of the prolactin receptor by molecular modeling. J Neurosci 21: 3207-3214.
  14. Blume A, Torner L, Liu Y, Subburaju S, Aguilera G, et al. (2009) Prolactin activates mitogen-activated protein kinase signaling and corticotropin releasing hormone transcription in rat hypothalamic neurons. Endocrinology 150: 1841-1849.
  15. Sakai T, O’Flaherty D, Giesecke AH, Kudo A, Hirota K, et al. (1995)Effects of total intravenous anesthesia with propofol on immune-endocrine changes during surgical stress. J Anaesth 9: 214-219.
  16. Marana E, Colicci S, Meo F, Marana R, Proietti R (2010) Neuroendocrine stress response in gynecological laparoscopy: TIVA with propofol versus sevoflurane anesthesia. J ClinAnesth 22: 250-255.
  17. Desborough JP (2000) The stress response to trauma and surgery. Br J Anaesth 85: 109-117.
  18. Mujagic Z, Cicko E, Verag-Brozovic V, Prašo M (2008) Serum level of cortisol and prolactin in patients treated under total intravenous anesthesia with propofol-fentanyl and under balanced anesthesia with isoflurane-fentanyl. Cent Eur J Med 3: 459-463.
  19. Lyons FM, Meeran K (1997) The physiology of the endocrine system. IntAnesthesiolClin 35: 1-21.
  20. Nicholson G, Burrin JM, Hall GM (1998) Peri-operative steroid supplementation. Anaesthesia 53: 1091-1104.
  21. Clevenger CV, Russell DH, Appasamy PM, Prystowsky MB (1990) Regulation of interleukin 2-driven T-lymphocyte proliferation by prolactin. ProcNatlAcadSci U S A 87: 6460-6464.
  22. Sheeran P, Hall GM (1997) Cytokines in anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 78: 201-219.
  23. Jameson P, Desborough JP, Bryant AE, Hall GM (1997) The effect of cortisol suppression on interleukin-6 and white blood cell responses to surgery. ActaAnaesthesiolScand 41: 304-308.
  24. Kehlet H (1997) Multimodal approach to control postoperative pathophysiology and rehabilitation. Br J Anaesth 78: 606-617.
  25. Kahveci K, Ornek D, Doger C, Aydin GB, Aksoy M, et al. (2014) The effect of anesthesia type on stress hormone response: comparison of general versus epidural anesthesia. Niger J ClinPract 17: 523-527.
  26. Hall GM, Young C, Holdcroft A, Alaghband-Zadeh J (1978) Substrate mobilisation during surgery. A comparison between halothane and fentanyl anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 33: 924-930.
  27. Bent JM, Paterson JL, Mashiter K, Hall GM (1984) Effects of high-dose fentanyl anaesthesia on the established metabolic and endocrine response to surgery. Anaesthesia 39: 19-23.
  28. Klingstedt C, Giesecke K, Hamberger B, Järnberg PO (1987) High- and low-dose fentanyl anaesthesia: circulatory and plasma catecholamine responses during cholecystectomy. Br J Anaesth 59: 184-188.
  29. Lacoumenta S, Paterson JL, Myers MA, Hall GM (1986) Effects of cortisol suppression by etomidate on changes in circulating metabolites associated with pelvic surgery. ActaAnaesthesiolScand 30: 101-104.
  30. Jung SM, Cho CK (2015) The effects of deep and light propofol anesthesia on stress response in patients undergoing open lung surgery: a randomized controlled trial. Korean J Anesthesiol 68: 224-231.
  31. Kaushal RP, Vatal A, Pathak R (2015) Effect of etomidate and propofol induction on hemodynamic and endocrine response in patients undergoing coronary artery bypass grafting/mitral valve and aortic valve replacement surgery on cardiopulmonary bypass. Ann Card Anaesth 18: 172-178.
  32. Wang XW, Cao JB, Lv BS, Mi WD, Wang ZQ, et al. (2015) Effect of Perioperative Dexmedetomidine on the Endocrine Modulators of Stress Response: A Meta-Analysis. ClinExpPharmacolPhysiol .
  33. McEwen BS (1994) Corticosteroids and hippocampal plasticity. Ann N Y AcadSci 746: 134-142.
  34. Roozendaal B, Quirarte GL, McGaugh JL (1997) Stress-activated hormonal systems and the regulation of memory storage. Ann N Y AcadSci 821: 247-258.
  35. Pavlides C, McEwen BS (1999) Effects of mineralocorticoid and glucocorticoid receptors on long-term potentiation in the CA3 hippocampal field. Brain Res 851: 204-214.
  36. Diamond DM, Bennett MC, Fleshner M, Rose GM (1992) Inverted-U relationship between the level of peripheral corticosterone and the magnitude of hippocampal primed burst potentiation. Hippocampus 2: 421-430.
  37. Han JS, Bizon JL, Chun HJ, Maus CE, Gallagher M (2002) Decreased glucocorticoid receptor mRNA and dysfunction of HPA axis in rats after removal of the cholinergic innervation to hippocampus. Eur J Neurosci 16: 1399-1404.
  38. Patel A, Bulloch K (2003) Type II glucocorticoid receptor immunoreactivity in the mossy cells of the rat and the mouse hippocampus. Hippocampus 13: 59-66.
  39. Kirschbaum C, Wolf OT, May M, Wippich W, Hellhammer DH (1996) Stress- and treatment-induced elevations of cortisol levels associated with impaired declarative memory in healthy adults. Life Sci 58: 1475-1483.
  40. de Quervain DJ, Roozendaal B, Nitsch RM, McGaugh JL, Hock C (2000) Acute cortisone administration impairs retrieval of long-term declarative memory in humans. Nat Neurosci 3: 313-314.
  41. Plihal W, Born J (1997) Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on declarative and procedural memory. J CognNeurosci 9: 534-547.
  42. Plihal W, Born J (1999) Effects of early and late nocturnal sleep on priming and spatial memory. Psychophysiology 36: 571-582.
  43. Battaglia FP, Sutherland GR, McNaughton BL (2004) Hippocampal sharp wave bursts coincide with neocortical "up-state" transitions. Learn Mem 11: 697-704.
  44. Paller KA, Voss JL (2004) Memory reactivation and consolidation during sleep. Learn Mem 11: 664-670.
  45. Nadel L, Payne JD (2002) The relationship between episodic memory and context: clues from memory errors made while under stress. Physiol Res 51 Suppl 1: S3-11.
  46. Gray MJ, Lombardo TW (2001) Complexity of trauma narratives as an index of fragmented memory in PTSD: A critical analysis. ApplCognPsychol 15: S171-S186.
  47. Bremner JD (1999) Does stress damage the brain? Biol Psychiatry 45: 797-805.
  48. Payne JD, Nadel L, Britton WB, Jacobs WJ (2003) The biopsychology of trauma and memory. In: Reisberg D, Hertel P (eds) Memory and emotion. Oxford University Press, London.
  49. Starkman MN, Gebarski SS, Berent S, Schteingart DE (1992) Hippocampal formation volume, memory dysfunction, and cortisol levels in patients with Cushing's syndrome. Biol Psychiatry 32: 756-765.
  50. Mauri M, Sinforiani E, Bono G, Vignati F, Berselli ME, et al. (1993) Memory impairment in Cushing's disease. ActaNeurolScand 87: 52-55.
  51. Kim JJ, Diamond DM (2002) The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nat Rev Neurosci 3: 453-462.
  52. Lupien SJ, de Leon M, de Santi S, Convit A, Tarshish C, et al. (1998) Cortisol levels during human aging predict hippocampal atrophy and memory deficits. Nat Neurosci 1: 69-73.
  53. Joëls M, Krugers HJ (2007) LTP after stress: up or down? Neural Plast 2007: 93202.
  54. Mangurian LP, Walsh RJ, Posner BI (1992) Prolactin enhancement of its own uptake at the choroid plexus. Endocrinology 131: 698-702.
  55. Walsh RJ, Slaby FJ, Posner BI (1987) A receptor-mediated mechanism for the transport of prolactin from blood to cerebrospinal fluid. Endocrinology 120: 1846-1850.
  56. Ben-Jonathan N, Mershon JL, Allen DL, Steinmetz RW (1996) Extrapituitary prolactin: distribution, regulation, functions, and clinical aspects. Endocr Rev 17: 639-669.
  57. Clapp C, Torner L, Gutiérrez-Ospina G, Alcántara E, López-Gómez FJ, et al. (1994) The prolactin gene is expressed in the hypothalamic-neurohypophyseal system and the protein is processed into a 14-kDa fragment with activity like 16-kDa prolactin. ProcNatlAcadSci U S A 91: 10384-10388.
  58. Emanuele NV, Jurgens JK, Halloran MM, Tentler JJ, Lawrence AM, et al. (1992) The rat prolactin gene is expressed in brain tissue: detection of normal and alternatively spliced prolactin messenger RNA. MolEndocrinol 6: 35-42.
  59. Nahi F, Arbogast LA (2003) Prolactin modulates hypothalamic preproenkephalin, but not proopiomelanocortin, gene expression during lactation. Endocrine 20: 115-122.
  60. Aceto P, Lai C, Perilli V, Dello Russo C, Federico B, et al. (2013) Stress-related biomarkers of dream recall and implicit memory under anaesthesia. Anaesthesia 68: 1141-1147.
  61. Bruhn J, Myles PS, Sneyd R, Struys MM (2006) Depth of anaesthesia monitoring: what's available, what's validated and what's next? Br J Anaesth 97: 85-94.
  62. Takamatsu I, Ozaki M, Kazama T (2006) Entropy indices vs the bispectral index for estimating nociception during sevofluraneanaesthesia. Br J Anaesth 96: 620-626.
  63. Chen X, Thee C, Gruenewald M, Ilies C, Höcker J, et al. (2012) Correlation of surgical pleth index with stress hormones during propofol-remifentanilanaesthesia. ScientificWorldJournal 2012: 879158.
  64. Aceto P, Perilli V, Lai C, Sacco T, Ancona P, et al. (2013) Update on post-traumatic stress syndrome after anesthesia. Eur Rev Med PharmacolSci 17: 1730-1737.
  65. Cerejeira J, Batista P, Nogueira V, Vaz-Serra A, Mukaetova-Ladinska EB (2013) The stress response to surgery and postoperative delirium: evidence of hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis hyperresponsiveness and decreased suppression of the GH/IGF-1 Axis. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol 26: 185-194.
  66. Aceto P, Perilli V, Lai C, Ciocchetti P, Vitale F, et al. (2015) Postoperative cognitive dysfunction after liver transplantation. Gen Hosp Psychiatry 37: 109-115.
  67. Rasmussen LS, O'Brien JT, Silverstein JH, Johnson TW, Siersma VD, et al. (2005) Is peri-operative cortisol secretion related to post-operative cognitive dysfunction? ActaAnaesthesiolScand 49: 1225-1231.
  68. Tang JX, Mardini F, Janik LS, Garrity ST, Li RQ, et al. (2013) Modulation of murine Alzheimer pathogenesis and behavior by surgery. Ann Surg 257: 439-448.
  69. Mashour GA, Shanks A, Tremper KK, Kheterpal S, Turner CR, et al. (2012) Prevention of intraoperative awareness with explicit recall in an unselected surgical population: a randomized comparative effectiveness trial. Anesthesiology 117: 717-725.
  70. Casati A, Fanelli G, Pietropaoli P, Proietti R, Tufano R, et al. (2007) Monitoring cerebral oxygen saturation in elderly patients undergoing general abdominal surgery: a prospective cohort study. Eur J Anaesthesiol 24: 59-65.
  71. Casati A, Fanelli G, Pietropaoli P, Proietti R, Tufano R, et al. (2005) Continuous monitoring of cerebral oxygen saturation in elderly patients undergoing major abdominal surgery minimizes brain exposure to potential hypoxia. Anesth Analg101:740-747.
Citation: Paola A, Carlo L, Cinzia DR, Valter P, Pierluigi N, et al. (2015) Stress Response to Surgery, Anesthetics Role and Impact on Cognition. J Anesth Clin Res 6:539.

Copyright: © 2015 Paola A, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top