GET THE APP

Perceived Positive Reinforcements Experienced by College Students
Journal of Alcoholism & Drug Dependence

Journal of Alcoholism & Drug Dependence
Open Access

ISSN: 2329-6488

+44 1223 790975

Research Article - (2013) Volume 1, Issue 2

Perceived Positive Reinforcements Experienced by College Students When Combining Alcohol and Energy Drinks

Weston S Kensinger and Amanda L Divin*
Department of Health Sciences, western illinois university, One University Circle, Macomb, Illinois, USA
*Corresponding Author: Amanda L Divin, Department of Health Sciences, western illinois university, One University Circle, Macomb, Illinois, USA, Tel: 309-298-1076 Email:

Abstract

Combining alcohol with energy drinks has become a common practice among college students. Although much research has focused on expectancies and/or negative consequences of such use, little has examined possible consequences. Because positive consequences may be more predominant than negative consequences when examining the immediate positive reinforcement of drinking behaviors, the role such reinforcement plays in the formation of alcohol abuse and dependence if unknown. Therefore the purpose of this study was to (1) To explore perceived positive reinforcements (PPRs) experienced by drinkers when consuming alcohol-only and combining alcohol with energy drinks and (2) Investigate relationship between PPRs and quantity of drinks.

Methods: A convenience sample of 371, 18-24 year old college students completed an online survey containing demographic questions, quantity/frequency index, and the Positive drinking consequences questionnaire for alcoholonly and combined use.

Results: Combined Users (CUs) reported significantly more PPR(s) than alcohol-only users (AOUs), but reported more PPRs when consuming alcohol-only. PPR(s) did not significantly increase with greater quantities of alcohol in AOUs, but did increase among CUs when consuming alcohol-only and combined drinks.

Conclusions: CU derived significantly more PPR(s) than AOUs, suggesting they hold expectancies and experience outcomes different from those of AOUs. Thus CUs may represent a special population at higher risk for alcohol abuse and dependence. Incorporating PPR(s) into current alcohol treatment and prevention practices represents an unexplored avenue with great potential to transform the college drinking culture.

Keywords: Perceived positive reinforcements; Positive consequences; Combined use; Alcohol and energy drink use; College students

Introduction

Caffeine and alcohol are two of the most historically used and abused substances. The first records of caffeine consumption date back to 2737 BC [1,2], while the history of alcohol use dates back to 6000 BC [3,4]. Today approximately 80-90% of all Americans regularly consume caffeine [5,6] while 52% of Americans ages 12 or older report being current alcohol consumers [7]. Because of their widespread use, addictive nature, and potential (negative) societal affects, a multitude of research has been performed regarding each of these two substances.

Overall results of such research indicate that college students are a special group of both caffeine and alcohol consumers whose prevalence of use easily exceeds that of other age groups. In fact, 89% of college students report caffeine consumption in the past 30 days [8] and 60.8% report being current alcohol drinkers [7]. Although things like coffee, tea, and/or cola have traditionally been the main sources of caffeine, since the introduction of Redbull© in the United Stated in 1997 [9], energy drinks (EDs) have become increasingly prevalent sources of caffeine, with 25% to 51% of college students reporting regular ED use [10,11]. The practice of combining alcohol with EDs has also become increasingly common among college students [10-14]. Unfortunately, it has also consistently been associated with a variety of dangerous consequences. For example, those who combine alcohol and EDs (combined users; CUs) are more likely to drink greater amounts of alcohol [12,15] and engage in heavy episodic drinking [10,12,15,16] than alcohol only users (AOUs). They underestimate their own level of impairment [15,17], as well as the impairment in others [12], resulting in a diminished ability to assess risk, poor judgment, and subsequent injury [18]. Negative alcohol-related consequences faced by CUs include being more likely to put themselves in physical danger while drunk [19], experience or commit a sexual assault [10], ride with an intoxicated driver [10,12], and drive home after drinking [12,19,20]. Of even greater concern is that while alcohol consumption increases reaction time (e.g. makes it greater) and the number of errors in performance, when an individual combines alcohol with EDs, caffeine offsets the increase in reaction time, allowing individuals to respond just as quickly as if they were not intoxicated. Although the amount of errors in their performance remains unchanged [21] the alertness may lead individuals to perceive themselves as less intoxicated and/or impaired. This inaccurate assessment of impairment may make CUs an even greater risk to themselves and others [15].

Research examining alcohol expectancies has found that expectations regarding the effects of alcohol are formed long before an individual takes his/her first sip of alcohol [22], and that such expectations significantly affect his/her motivation to drink as well as the actual effects alcohol has on his/her behavior [23-25]. Numerous studies have examined the role that alcohol expectancies have in drinking behavior. Such expectancies are the beliefs that one holds about what will happen if they consume alcohol [26]. Twenty years of research suggest that beliefs about what will happen when consuming alcohol (expectancies) are reliably associated with drinking behavior [27]. The more positive alcohol expectancies an individual holds, the more likely it is that he/she will engage in drinking behavior. Studies suggest that higher quantities of drinking (4+ drinks for women, 6+ drinks for men) positively correlate with excitement and more positive expectancies [26]. The expectancies an individual has about drinking may correlate with his/her drinking behavior. In one study individuals who drank heavier amounts on an infrequent basis (binge drinkers) had higher expectations for increased assertiveness and cognitive enhancement. When subjects consumed drinking less alcohol per drinking episode, they reported more negative expectancies [28]. Individuals who exhibit positive alcoholexpectancies are associated with heavy drinking, more frequent alcohol use, and associated alcohol related problems [29].

Research examining caffeine expectancies has had similar results [30-32]. Thus an individual’s expectancies, combined with the drive to obtain them [33], the desirability of the substance’s effects [31,34,35], the pharmacological properties of the drug (alcohol, caffeine, or combination of the two), and an individual’s past experiences may provide insight to his/her drinking behaviors [23,30-32].

Although a multitude of research has focused on alcohol and caffeine expectancies, very little has examined positive consequences or outcomes of their use. Whereas expectancies are anticipated, general beliefs that certain types of positive or negative consequences/outcomes are likely to occur as a result of alcohol consumption [23,36,37], perceived positive reinforcements (PPRs) are the actual, real-life positive consequences/outcomes experienced by the individual while drinking alcohol [36]. Those who combine alcohol and EDs (combined users; CUs) do report experiencing more positive consequences or outcomes associated with their combined consumption such as increased wakefulness and energy [16,17,36,38] less mental fatigue, more feelings of stimulation (e.g. elation, energy, stimulated, talkative, up, vigorous instead of; [15], enhanced intensity of a sexual experience [36], fewer headaches, less weakness, decreased dry mouth [17], less mental fatigue [15], and enhanced social interactions [16,36,38]. Consequently, experiencing such positive consequences associated with one’s combined use may reinforce the behavior [39,40]. According to addiction experts, PPRs may be a good predictor of an individual becoming psychologically and/or physically dependent on alcohol because when a person believes that he/she needs alcohol to enhance who he/she is (e.g., to be more social) or to helphim/her achieve positive outcomes (e.g., better chance with the opposite sex) experiencing such outcomes rewards the drinking behavior thus acting to positively reinforce it [41].

While research on ED consumption and combined use continues to increase, more is needed to fully understand the implications of combined use within the college population. More specifically, it is important that researchers better understand the rationale for combining alcohol and EDs, as well as the actual experiences of the drinker, if successful interventions are to be created to address this potentially deadly behavior. Currently very little research has examined the PPRs experienced by individuals when consuming alcohol [39,40,42-45], and no published research has specifically examined PPRs reported from the combination of alcohol and EDs. Because positive outcomes may be more predominant than negative outcomes, especially when examining the immediate positive reinforcement of drinking behaviors [39,46], and only a limited number of studies have examined the positive consequences of drinking within a college population, the role and extent that such PPR(s) play in drinking behavior has not been established. This study fills an important gap by not only asking college-aged drinkers about the PPR(s) experienced when consuming alcohol only, but also about the PPR(s) experienced when combining alcohol and EDs.

However, since this study is the first of its kind, we focused on basic elements of PPR(s). More specifically, the aims of the current study were to: (1) examine the PPR(s) experienced by college students when consuming alcohol-only and the combination of alcohol and EDs and (2) explore if differences in PPR(s) exist based on user type and/or gender.

Materials and Methods

Study population

A convenience sample of 18-26 year old college students enrolled in undergraduate courses at Oklahoma State University and/or Northern Oklahoma College were solicited for voluntary participation in this study by professors and teaching assistants. Participants were directed to complete an anonymous and confidential Internet-based online survey asking about their alcohol and ED consumption, as well as the motives for use and perceived effects of both alcohol and the combination of alcohol and EDs. Prior to beginning the study, Institutional Review Board [IRB] approval was obtained from Oklahoma State University, and in accordance with the IRB, informed consent was obtained from all participants prior to participation.

Measures

Demographic characteristics: Participants self-reported their age, sex, ethnicity, and year in school. Information regarding place of residence (on/off campus) and involvement in the Greek system (yes/no) was also collected. Additional factors identified by previous research as influencing alcohol and/or ED use were also collected (e.g. access to alcohol (yes/no), access to EDs (yes/no), and if ever combined alcohol and energy drinks (yes/no)).

Amount and frequency of use: The amount and frequency of use was collected with a Quantity-Frequency Index (QFI). A QFI for use of alcohol only was first completed, with participants answering the following questions: “In the last 30 days, how many occasions have you drank alcohol?”, “On average, how many standard alcoholic drinks do you consume on a drinking occasion?”, “Over the last 30 days, how many times (if any) have you had five or more drinks in a row?” and “In the past 30 days, what was the greatest number of alcoholic drinks you consumed in a row? Over how many hours did you consume alcohol on this occasion?” The alcohol only QFI clearly defines a drink as a 1.5 ounce of 80 proof liquor (a ‘shot’), 12 ounces of beer, or 4-5 ounces of wine, with one 750 ml bottle of 80 proof liquor equaling 17 drinks, and gives a visual image of each.

A QFI for the combined use of alcohol and EDs was then collected with participants answering the following questions: “In the last 30 days, how many occasions have you combined alcohol and energy drinks?”, “On average, how many standard alcoholic drinks do you consume on a combined-use drinking occasion?”, “On average, how many standard energy drinks (8oz.ED or 2oz. energy shot) do you consume on a combined-use drinking occasion?”, “In the last 30 days, how many times have you consumed had 3 or more combined drinks in a row?,” “While combining in the last 30 days, what was the greatest number of alcoholic drinks you consumed in a row? Over how many hours did you combine on this occasion?” and “While combining in the last 30 days, what is the greatest number of energy drinks you consumed in a row? Over how many hours did you combine on this occasion?” The combined use QFI states: “Combined-use is either mixing energy drinks with alcohol or using an energy drink (ED) within plus or minus 2 hours of using alcohol. For example, if you consume energy drinks before going out to the bar, and then start drinking alcohol, this is still considered combined-use,” and then gives the following text next to a visual image of the product: “8 oz of standard energy drinks (Red Bull)=1 Energy Drink”, “2 oz Energy Shot=1 Energy Drink”, “16 oz Standard Energy Drinks (Monster, Full Throttle, Rockstar, etc.)=2 Energy Drinks”, “16 oz (NOS)=3 Energy Drinks” and “8 oz (Spike, Redline)=3 Energy Drinks.”

Perceived positive reinforcement(s): Perceived Positive Reinforcement(s) (PPRs) were measured using a modified version of the Positive Drinking Consequences Questionnaire (PDCQ; [36]). The PDCQ is comprised of 14 questions which ask participants to indicate the number of times they have experienced the specific listed consequences in the last three months, stressing that they only report what actually occurred, not what they had thought to occur. The original PDCQ is scored using a five point interval scale based on the number of times subjects had experienced the consequence (1, 1-2, 3-5, 6-10, >10). Scores on the 14 items are then summed, with a possible range from 14-70, giving an overall measure of PPR for alcohol consumption. In the current study, the response categories were modified to be “(1) Almost never”, “(2) Some of the time”, “(3) Half of the time”, “(4) Most of the time”, and “(5) Almost always”. An overall sum score was calculated for each participant. As with the original PDCQ, higher scores on the modified instrument used in the current study represent experiencing greater PPR(s) of alcohol consumption or combined consumption of alcohol and EDs.

Participants completed two PDCQ’s, one PDCQ based on alcohol only consumption and a separate PDCQ based on the combined consumption of alcohol and EDs.

Data collection

Due to the sensitive nature of the information collected and possible legal consequences of underage alcohol consumption, the use of an anonymous and confidential Internet-based online survey was used to collect data. If participation was desired, participants were directed to a web address to complete the survey which included demographic questions, the alcohol-only QFI, the combined use QFI, the alcohol-only PDCQ, and the combined use PDCQ. Data collection occurred during the fall academic semester of 2009.

Data analysis

All analyses were performed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences SPSS version 16.0. Participants were first categorized for analysis based on their responses to “Do you drink alcohol?” and “Have you combined alcohol and energy drinks?” Those who responded “No” to “Do you drink alcohol?” were classified as non-drinkers and excluded from the final sample.

Those who responded “Yes” to “Do you drink alcohol?” comprised the final sample. Of the final sample, those who responded “No” to “Have you combined alcohol and energy drinks?” were classified as alcohol only users (AOUs), while those who responded “Yes” were classified as combined users (CUs). Mean total scores for the PDCQ were calculated, as were scores for individual items on the PDCQ. Independent samples t-tests were used to measures differences in PPR(s) between AOUs and CUs and paired samples t-tests were used to measure differences in PPR(s) within the CU group. Pearson product-moment correlations were calculated to determine the relationship between the average amount of alcohol consumed on one occasion, overall PPR and individual PPR items among CUs. Alpha level was set at p ≤ 0.05 for all statistical analysis.In the event of missing data, cases were excluded pairwise, meaning they were excluded only from the analyses for which that data was necessary but included in other analyses for which they did provide data.

Results

Demographics

A total of 540 students completed the survey; however this number was reduced to include only participants of traditional college age (18-24) and those who consumed alcohol,leaving a final sample consisting of 371 participants. Table 1 provides a summary of demographic characteristics of the overall sample and separated by gender.

Group Total** Males Females
Age      
18-19 135 45 85
20-21 152 43 109
22-23 66 28 38
24-25 8 5 3
Academic Classification      
Freshman 98 40 54
Sophomore 82 28 53
Junior 77 19 58
Senior 106 39 67
Graduate Student 7 2 5
Race      
White/Non-Hispanic 312 108 200
African American 15 4 11
Hispanic 17 7 10
Asian/Pacific Islander 3 1 1
American Indian 16 6 10
Other 6 1 5
Current Living Arrangement      
On Campus 129 46 80
Off Campus 236 79 155
Greek Membership      
Yes 84 22 61
No 260 96 160
Combined Alcohol and Energy Drinks    
Yes 195 72 123
No 170 56 114
*Includes only participants reporting alcohol consumption
**Number does not equal number appearing in ‘Total’ column owning to non-response

Table 1: Demographic Characteristics of Final Sample*.

Perceived positive reinforcement(s)

Perceived positive reinforcement(s) among all alcohol users: Table 2 presents the means and standard deviations for the alcohol-only PDCQ as well as alcohol-only PDCQ individual items for the entire sample and separated by gender. The entire sample, which included both AOUs and CUs, had a total alcohol-only PDCQ score of 31.27 ± 10.22. When separated by gender, the score for males was 32.51 ± 10.82 and the score for females was 30.58 ± 9.91. Overall alcohol-only PDCQ scores were not significantly different between males and females. However significant differences did exist on three individual items (“I found myself in a frightening situation and I felt surprisingly fearless”, “I found a creativesolution to a problem I might otherwise have had difficulty solving”, and “The intensity of a sexual experience was enhanced”) with males scoring significantly higher than females. These findings suggest that males who consume alcohol may experience greater PPRs from their alcohol use than females; and when males consume alcohol they experience significantly greater reinforcement from their alcohol use when it comes to feeling fearless, being creative, and/or experiencing greater intensity of a sexual encounter.

  Total Sample Male Female
Individual Item from PDCQ      
“I approached a person that I probably wouldn’t      
have spoken to otherwise” 2.57 ± 1.11 2.54±1.05 2.59 ± 1.15
“I told a funny story or joke and make others      
laugh” 2.71 ± 1.18 2.76±1.23 2.68 ±1.15
“I revealed a personal feeling or emotion that I had      
previously kept secret” 2.07 ± 1.03 2.10±1.00 2.04 ± 1.04
“I felt like I had enough energy to stay out all night      
partying or dancing” 2.63 ± 1.21 2.69±1.20 2.57 ± 1.20
“In a situation in which I would usually have      
stayed quiet, I found it easy to make conversation” 2.80 ± 1.13 2.83±1.11 2.79 ± 1.17
“I stood up for a friend or confronted someone      
who was in the wrong” 2.21 ± 1.14 2.28±1.15 2.17± 1.14
“I found myself in a frightening situation and I felt      
surprisingly fearless” 1.73 ± 1.03 1.94 ±1.09** 1.61 ± 0.91
“I found a creative solution to a problem I might      
otherwise have had difficulty solving” 1.77 ± 0.941 1.97 ±0.98** 1.65 ± 0.91
“I felt especially confident that other people found      
me attractive” 2.22 ± 1.10 2.32±1.14 2.17 ± 1.09
“The intensity of a sexual experience was      
enhanced” 2.25 ± 1.23 2.48 ±1.25* 2.13 ± 1.21
“I acted out a sexual fantasy that I might ordinarily      
be embarrassed to reveal or attempt” 1.78 ± 1.08 1.90±1.18 1.70 ± 1.02
“On a particularly stressful day, I noticed a release      
of tension from my muscles and nerves” 2.27 ± 1.15 2.35±1.15 2.23 ± 1.13
“Something that would have ordinarily made me      
upset or emotional didn’t really get me down” 1.92 ± 0.989 2.02±1.04 1.85 ± 0.95
“Things that I had been worrying about all day no      
longer seemed important” 2.40 ± 1.194 2.41±1.18 2.39 ± 1.20
Overall Total Score 31.27 ± 10.22 32.51±10.82 30.58 ± 9.91
**p<.01; *p<.05

Table 2: Mean ± Standard Deviation for Overall Perceived Positive Reinforcement and Individual Perceived Positive Reinforcements in Total Sample and Separated by Gender.

Table 3 presents the means and standard deviations for the alcohol-only PDCQ as well as alcohol-only PDCQ individual items for all alcohol users separated by AOUs and CUs. An independent t-test found a statistically significant difference in PPR between CUs (M=32.91, SD=10.13) and AOUs (M=29.35, SD=10.00); t (352)=3.30, p<0.001 (two-tailed). CUs scored higher on the alcohol-only PDCQ than the AOUs, suggesting that CUs derived moreoverall PPR than AOUs. When examining individual items of the alcohol-only PDCQ, CUs scored significantly higher on the items “I approached a person that I probably wouldn’t have spoken to otherwise”, “I revealed a personal feeling or emotion that I had previously kept secret”, “I felt like I had enough energy to stay out all night partying or dancing”, “I found myself in a frightening situation and I felt surprisingly fearless”, “I felt especially confident that other people found me attractive”, “The intensity of a sexual experience was enhanced”, “On a particularly stressful day, I noticed a release of tension from my muscles and nerves” and “Things that I had been worrying about all day no longer seemed important”. Higher scores on these items by CUs suggests that CUs perceive significantly greater reinforcement from their alcohol use than AOUs, and that alcohol use enables them to be more outgoing, open with their emotions, energetic, fearless, and attractive, experience greater intensity during a sexual encounter, and reduced tension and worries.

Individual Item from PDCQ Alcohol-Only Users Combined Users
“I approached a person that I probably wouldn’t have    
spoken to otherwise” ± 1.09** 2.74 ± 1.11
“I told a funny story or joke and make others laugh” 2.61± 1.13 2.80 ± 1.21
“I revealed a personal feeling or emotion that I had    
previously kept secret” ±0.94** 2.21 ± 1.07
“I felt like I had enough energy to stay out all night partying    
or dancing” ± 1.18** 2.82 ± 1.20
“In a situation in which I would usually have stayed quiet, I    
found it easy to make conversation” ± 1.23 2.82 ± 1.07
“I stood up for a friend or confronted someone who was in    
the wrong” ± 1.134 2.27 ± 1.15
“I found myself in a frightening situation and I felt    
surprisingly fearless” ± 0.94** 1.85± 1.08
“I found a creative solution to a problem I might otherwise    
have had difficulty solving” ± 0.85 1.84 ± 1.00
“I felt especially confident that other people found me    
attractive” ± 1.08* 2.33 ± 1.12
“The intensity of a sexual experience was enhanced” 2.09± 1.21* 2.39 ± 1.25
“I acted out a sexual fantasy that I might ordinarily be 1.66± 1.03  
embarrassed to reveal or attempt”   1.88 ± 1.12
“On a particularly stressful day, I noticed a release of    
tension from my muscles and nerves” 1.98± 1.02** 2.52 ± 1.18
“Something that would have ordinarily made me upset or 1.82± 0.94  
emotional didn’t really get me down”   1.99 ± 1.01
“Things that I had been worrying about all day no longer    
seemed important” 2.24± 1.15* 2.54 ± 1.21
Overall Total Score 29.35 ±10.00 ** 32.91 ±10.13
**p<.01; *p<.05

Table 3: Mean ± Standard Deviation for Overall Perceived Positive Reinforcement and Individual Perceived Positive Reinforcements in Alcohol-Only Users and Combined Users.

Perceived positive reinforcement(s) among combined users: Table 4 presents the means and standarddeviations for the combined use PDCQ as well as combined use PDCQ individual items for all CUs. Participants in this analysis were limited to those who were CUs (n=184). The total combined use PDCQ score among CUs was 25 ± 13.12. When separated by gender, the score for males was 26.53 ± 14.49 and the score for females was 24.09 ± 12.12. Overall combined use PDCQ scores were not significantly different between male and female CUs (t(182)=1.22, p<0.223 (two-tailed)). However significant differences were found on one individual combined use PDCQ item (“I found myself in a frightening situation and I felt surprisingly fearless”), with males scoring significantly higher than females. These findings suggest that although overall PPR from combined use does not differ between males andfemales, males experience greater positive reinforcement for their combined use when it comes to fearlessness.

Individual Item from PDCQ All Combined Users Males (N=67) Females (N=117)
“I approached a person that I probably      
wouldn’t have spoken to otherwise” 1.92 ± 1.18 1.91± 1.15 1.93 ± 1.21
“I told a funny story or joke and make      
others laugh” 1.91 ± 1.17 1.97± 1.17 1.88 ± 1.18
“I revealed a personal feeling or emotion      
that I had previously kept secret” 1.63± 0.98 1.77± 1.07 1.54 ± 0.92
“I felt like I had enough energy to stay out      
all night partying or dancing” 2.22 ± 1.43 2.18± 1.34 2.24 ± 1.47
“In a situation in which I would usually      
have stayed quiet, I found it easy to make      
conversation” 1.96 ± 1.94 2.07± 1.19 1.91 ± 1.21
“I stood up for a friend or confronted      
someone who was in the wrong” 1.78 ± 1.16 1.97± 1.29 1.67 ± 1.08
“I found myself in a frightening situation      
and I felt surprisingly fearless” 1.61 ± 1.05 1.87 ± 1.22* 1.46 ± 0.91
“I found a creative solution to a problem I      
might otherwise have had difficulty      
solving” 1.55 ± 0.91 1.68± 1.03 1.47 ± 0.83
“I felt especially confident that other      
people found me attractive” 1.72 ± 1.06 1.82± 1.15 1.66 ± 1.00
“The intensity of a sexual experience was      
enhanced” 1.74 ± 1.14 1.87± 1.26 1.65 ± 1.58
“I acted out a sexual fantasy that I might      
ordinarily be embarrassed to reveal or      
attempt” 1.58± 1.04 1.73± 1.14 1.47 ± 0.95
“On a particularly stressful day, I noticed a      
release of tension from my muscles and      
nerves” 1.82 ± 1.18 1.86± 1.20 1.78± 1.15
“Something that would have ordinarily      
made me upset or emotional didn’t really      
get me down” 1.68 ± 1.04 1.77± 1.15 1.61 ± 0.97
“Things that I had been worrying about all      
day no longer seemed important” 1.82 ± 1.15 1.79± 1.16 1.83 ± 1.14
Overall Total Score 25 ± 13.12 26.53± 14.49 24.09 ± 12.12
*p<.05

Table 4: Mean ± Standard Deviation for Overall Perceived Positive Reinforcement and Individual Perceived Positive Reinforcements in Combined Users, Separated by Gender.

Table 5 presents the means and standard deviations for the alcohol-only PDCQ, combined use PDCQ, and individual items of both PDCQs for all CUs. A paired-samples t-test found a statistically significant difference in the alcohol-only PDCQ scores (M=32.91, SD=10.29) and combined use PDCQ scores (M=25.06, SD=13.12); t(180)=8.24, p<0.000 (two-tailed) for CUs. This finding suggests that CUs derive more overall PPR from consuming alcohol alone rather than combining alcohol and EDs. Analysis of individual PDCQ items revealed that for all PPRs listed, CUs also experienced greater reinforcement when consuming alcohol-only rather than combining it with EDs.

Individual Item from PDCQ Consume Alcohol-Only Consume Alcohol and Energy Drinks
“I approached a person that I probably wouldn’t have    
spoken to otherwise” 2.74 ± 1.11*** 1.92 ± 1.18
“I told a funny story or joke and make others laugh” 2.80 ± 1.21*** 1.91 ± 1.17
“I revealed a personal feeling or emotion that I had    
previously kept secret” 2.21 ± 1.07*** 1.63 ± 0.98
“I felt like I had enough energy to stay out all night    
partying or dancing” 2.82 ± 1.20*** 2.22 ± 1.45
“In a situation in which I would usually have stayed    
quiet, I found it easy to make conversation” 2.82 ± 1.07*** 1.96 ± 1.94
“I stood up for a friend or confronted someone who    
was in the wrong” 2.27 ± 1.15*** 1.78 ± 1.16
“I found myself in a frightening situation and I felt    
surprisingly fearless” 1.85 ± 1.08*** 1.61 ± 1.05
“I found a creative solution to a problem I might    
otherwise have had difficulty solving” 1.84 ± 1.00*** 1.55 ± 0.91
“I felt especially confident that other people found    
me attractive” 2.33 ± 1.12*** 1.72 ± 1.06
“The intensity of a sexual experience was enhanced” 2.39 ± 1.25*** 1.74 ± 1.14
“I acted out a sexual fantasy that I might ordinarily    
be embarrassed to reveal or attempt” 1.88 ± 1.12*** 1.58 ± 1.04
“On a particularly stressful day, I noticed a release of    
tension from my muscles and nerves” 2.52 ± 1.19*** 1.82 ± 1.18
“Something that would have ordinarily made me    
upset or emotional didn’t really get me down” 1.99 ± 1.01*** 1.69 ± 1.04
“Things that I had been worrying about all day no    
longer seemed important” 2.54 ± 1.21*** 1.82 ± 1.15
Overall Total Score 32.91 ± 10.29*** 25 ± 13.12
***p<.0001

Table 5: Mean ± Standard Deviation for Overall Perceived Positive Reinforcement and Individual Perceived Positive Reinforcements in Combined Users-Comparison of Alcohol-Only and Combined Consumption.

Discussion

Consistent with previous research [39,40,42-45], drinkers did indeed experience ‘positive consequences’ or PPR(s) when consuming alcohol. The current study, however, extends the body of knowledge by (1) examining and comparing the PPR(s) experienced in two different types of drinkers (those who consumed alcohol only (AOUs) and those who combined alcohol and EDs (CUs)) and (2) comparing the PPR(s) experienced by CUs when consuming alcohol-only and combining alcohol with EDs. Results from the current study suggest CUs experienced significantly greater overall PPR than AOUs, both when they [CUs] consumed alcohol-only beverages and when they combined alcohol and EDs. Individual item analysis revealed that during alcohol-only consumption, CUs experienced the specific PPRs of being more outgoing, feeling more open with emotions, feeling energetic, fearless, and attractive, having an enhanced sexual experience, and experiencing a reduction of tension and worries atsignificantly greater amounts than AOUs. These results suggest CUs are a unique subset of drinkers among the college drinking population, as their perception of alcohol experience(s) are more reinforcing than those of AOUs. These finding are important because when college students hold positive expectancies for alcohol use and actually experience positive outcomes as a result of this use, the positive drinking experiences/outcomes reinforce the positive expectancies, which then causes students to experience further positive experiences/outcomes, resulting in a positive feedback cycle [39,44]. Thus experiencing such PPR(s) may encourage CUs to drink more often, at higher quantities, and for different reasons than AOUs.

Among CUs, one surprising finding emerged when comparing PPR(s) experienced during alcohol-only use and PPRs experienced during combined use. Our results suggest CUs experienced significantly greater overall PPR when consuming alcohol-only rather than when combining. Moreover, when comparing individual PPRs, CUs also experienced significantly greater PPRs when consuming alcohol-only. This finding is very interesting, because although it has been hypothesized that individuals may combine alcohol and EDs to blunt negative effects of alcohol [47], increase pleasurable aspects of alcohol [15], decrease effects of a hangover [48], or allow the drinker to manage his/her subjective experience of intoxication [49], in the current study, CUs choose to combine alcohol and EDs even though they reported experiencing greater PPR(s) when consuming alcohol-only. It begs the question, why would CUs combine if they find alcohol alone more reinforcing? These results suggest there are factors not measured in the present study which influence the decision to drink alcohol-only or to combine it with EDs. For example the current study did not look at the social or cultural environment and/or peer groups of participants. Kuntsche and Stewart [50] and Kuntsche et al. [51] found that drinkers conform to the standards and peer pressure of those around them. Thus participants may be inclined tochoose one beverage over another or to drink for different reasons based upon their peer group or social environment. Further research should explore such factors and the role they may play in combined-use drinking behavior [50,51].

The current study also examined if differences in PPR(s) existed based on gender. Although no significant difference in overall PPR was found between all alcohol consuming males and females in the study, males reported experiencing significantly greater amounts of the specific PPRs of feeling fearless, being creative, and having an enhanced sexual experience. When further examining PPR(s) among only CUs, no significant difference was found between males and females, although males did report experiencing significantly greater PPR in regard to feeling fearless. Previous research examining the role of gender in PPR(s) has produced mixed results [39,40,52]. More research is needed to uncover if this is a special case within our sample, or if when looking at gender within CUs, males and females experience identical PPR(s) when combining. Our results do, however, echo those of Park [39] in that generally speaking males and females are more alike in their experiences of PPR(s).

Our findings regarding the relationship between PPR(s), quantity of drinks, and type of drinks were also somewhat surprising. Among AOUs overall PPR did not increase with increasing number of drinks, but among CUs, whether consuming alcohol-only or combining alcohol and EDs, it did. This suggests that variables other than PPR, which were not measured in the current study, may contribute to the drinking behaviors of CUs. Previous studies have identified personality factors such as sensation seeking [19,53,54], risk-taking [20,55], and sexual risk taking [56] as correlates to ED consumption. Perhaps such underlying traits may contribute to the initial desire to consume EDs, and subsequent desire to combine alcohol and EDs. They may also somehow impact the drinking experience causing CUs to derivesignificantly greater overall PPR from any drinking experience, whether it involves alcohol-only or the combination of alcohol and EDs. The positive relationship between overall PPR, as well as each individual PPR, among CUs when combining alcohol and EDs, and quantity of drinks suggests that the addition of EDs to the drinking experience makes it significantly more reinforcing. Unfortunately the significantly greater amount of PPR(s) experienced with increasing quantity of beverages by CUs is troubling, as it may potentially lead to more episodes of binge drinking as well as more long-term negative consequences [42,45]. Further, although heavy alcohol drinkers may become more accurate at predicting their own alcohol-related consequences [44], the addition of EDs reduces the perception of intoxication [17], desensitizes the drinker to alcohol’s negative [47] or sedating [57] effects, enables the drinker to stay awake longer to consume more alcohol [58], and increases the rewarding aspects of alcohol consumption [15]; all things which may contribute to increased quantity of alcohol consumed,formation of positive expectancies, occurrence of positive experiences, overestimation of positive and underestimation of negative consequences of alcohol, and perpetuation of the alcohol abuse cycle [39,40,44]. Overall, these finding suggest CUs are a special population of drinkers who may hold a different perspective and experience when drinking, and thus require a different type of alcohol prevention and treatment programs than AOUs.

Limitations

Although the current study had many noteworthy findings, it is not without limitations. First, the current study used a convenience sample and cross-sectional data, thus a causal relationship cannot be determined. Additionally, due to the sensitive nature of the questions, sampling bias may have occurred, and data may underestimate the true prevalence of risky orundesirable behaviors. Efforts were made to minimize the sampling bias by using an anonymous web-based survey which participants could complete in private without the risk of legal ramifications. However the finding(s) regarding participants reporting increased positive reinforcement from the combination of alcohol and EDs is consistent with other studies which have examined alcohol [39,40] or EDs individually [59,60]. Second, approximately 54% of the sample who consumed alcohol reported being a CU. This is within the wide range of prevalence reported in the literature, although the sample was comprised of mainly Caucasian female college students, which limits the general ability. However, the large majority of research examining CU has found that CUs are more likely to be Caucasian and young [10], even among community samples [61]. Future research should include a larger, more diverse sample of college students, with participants from a variety of ethnic backgrounds, educational institutions including both private and public colleges and universities in different geographical regions, and a better balance of gender. It should also include other potential confounding and/or contributing factors other than demographics, such as social/cultural environment, peer groups, and/or personality factors, as potential influences to drinking behavior. Fourth, unfortunately no valid and reliable instrument currently exists which specifically examines combined-use in any capacity. The PDCQ, used in this study to measure PPR(s), was originally designed to measure alcohol-only use. While it showed acceptable reliability in our study (α=0.97), development of instruments specifically examining combined-use is necessary to provide more accurate results which may be more sensitive to characteristics of this population.

Conclusions and recommendations for future research

While a large body of research has examined alcohol use in college students and a growing body of research has examined alcohol and ED use, our study is the first to examine and compare the PPR(s) experienced in AOUs and CUs. It is further strengthened by examining the PPRs experienced by CUs under two conditions: when consuming alcohol-only and when consuming alcohol and EDs. Our results suggest CUs are a different type of drinker than AOUs. More specifically, CUs derive significantly greater PPR(s) from their drinking experiences, whether they consume alcohol-only or the combination of alcohol and EDs.

It has been suggested that PPR(s) be examined as an effective tool to help drinkers move toward healthier behaviors [36]. The findings of the present study support this suggestion and have important implications for researchers, public health workers, university administrators, policymakers, and the general public. Researchers should place increased focus on examining the combination of alcohol and any type of caffeine containing beverage, specifically comparing CUs and AOUs. This research will generate knowledge to assist educators and public health workers not only to educate college students on the many potential risks of combined use, but also how to differentiate between a “wide-awake drunk” and someone who is safe to conduct him/herself without assistance. Our findings also suggest CUs hold expectancies and experience outcomes different from those of AOUs. Thus they may require different messages or techniques. Future prevention, education, and treatment programs should be developed using intervention strategies such as Self-Determination-Theory or Motivational Interviewing, which recognize the importance of utilizing a individual’s past experiences (e.g. PPR(s)) and motivations to help him/her better foster an environment where health behavior change can be found. These strategies should be tailored to the unique needs and fallacious PPR(s) of CUs. Future prevention, education, and treatment programs should also utilize social norm campaignsto educate CUs about the realities of combined use. These should include both injunctive and behavioral norms to help change the peer culture [62].

Such programs may not help CUs to realize that their drinking view point and behavior are not typical of others, nor are the perceptions experienced while drinking. Because ED companies often use ‘student managers’ to distribute free samples of EDs on campus as well as to gather information about the campus culture to better tailor marketing efforts [10], university administrators may use this research to develop policies regarding the promotion and advertising of EDs on campus and at institutional events. Further, policy makers may wish to consider and reference this research when discussing potential advertising, labeling, and ingredient requirements of EDs and caffeinated alcoholic beverages, as false advertising claims such as “gives you wings”© may contribute to the fallacious perceptions reported by CUs.

Given the growing popularity of EDs, the unending use of alcohol by college students, the increasing consumption of combined beverages, and the known potentially negative consequences of such consumption, it is vital to understand the similarities and differences in college students who choose to consume alcohol-only and those who engage in combined-use in order to develop effective interventions. This study is one of the first to suggest that CUs are different from AOUs on a variety of levels. Our findings are a critical first step toward conceptualizing why these two groups engage in drinking behaviors. However PPRs must still be investigated further. The role they play in the formation of alcohol expectancies, outcomes, and habits, as well as what they reveal about the addictive nature of both alcohol and caffeine represent an area of untapped unlimited potential for the prevention and treatment of alcohol-related and other addictive disorders.

References

  1. Fredholm BB (2011) Notes on the history of caffeine use. Handb Exp Pharmacol 200: 1-9.
  2. National Institute of Mental Health (2008) Caffeine No Substitute for a Nap to Enhance Memory. Science Update.
  3. Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine-Behavioral Pharmacology Research Unit (2003) Use and Common Sources of Caffeine. Information about Caffeine Dependence.
  4. Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration (2012) Alcohol Use. Results from the 2011 National Survey on Drug Use and Health: Summary of National Findings
  5. Norton TR, Lazev AB, Sullivan MJ (2011) The ‘‘Buzz’’ on Caffeine: Patterns of Caffeine Use in a Convenience Sample of College Students. J Caffeine Res 1: 35-40.
  6. O’Brien M, McCoy T, Rhodes S, Wagoner A, Wolfson M (2008)Caffeinated cocktails: Energy drink consumption, high-risk drinking, and alcohol-related consequences among college students. Acad Emerg Med 15: 453-460.
  7. Malinauskas BM, Aeby VG, Overton RF, Carpenter-Aeby T, Barber-Heidal K (2007) A survey of energy drink consumption patterns among college students. Nutr J 6: 35.
  8. Brache K, Stockwell T (2011) Drinking patterns and risk behaviors associated with combined alcohol and energy drink consumption in college drinkers. Addict Behav 36: 1133-1140.
  9. Marczinski CA (2011) Alcohol mixed with energy drinks: Consumption patterns and motivations for use in U.S college students. Int J Environ Res Public Health 8: 3232-3245.
  10. Miller KE (2008) Energy drinks, race, and problem behaviors among college students. J Adolesc Health 43: 490-497.
  11. Marczinski CA, Fillmore MT, Bardgett ME, Howard MA (2011) Effects of energy drinks with alcohol on behavioral control: Risks for college students consuming trendy cocktails. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 35: 1282-1292.
  12. Marzell M (2011) Reducing high-risk college drinking: Examining the consumption of alcohol-energy drink cocktails among college students. Student Dissertation.
  13. Ferreira S, Mello M, Pompeia S, Souza-Formigoni M (2006) Effects of energy drink ingestion of alcohol intoxication. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 30: 598-605.
  14. Arria AM, O’Brien MC (2009) The Use of Caffeine in Alcoholic Beverages.
  15. Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Kasperski SJ, Vincent KB, Griffiths RR, et al. (2011) Energy drink consumption and increased risk for alcohol dependence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 35: 365-375.
  16. Thombs DL, O’Mara RJ, Tsukamoto M, Rossheim ME, Weiler RM, et al. (2010) Event-level analysis of energy drink consumption and alcohol intoxication in bar patrons. Addict Behav 35: 325-330.
  17. Marczinski CA,Fillmore MT (2003) Dissociative antagonistic effects of caffeine on alcohol-induced impairment of behavioral control. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 11: 228-236.
  18. Christiansen BA, Goldman MS, Inn A (1982) Development of alcohol–related expectancies in adolescents: Separating pharmacological from social–learning influences. J Consult Clin Psychol 50: 336-344.
  19. Brown SA, Goldman MS, Inn A, Anderson LR (1980) Expectations of reinforcement from alcohol: Their domain and relation to drinking patterns. J Consult Clin Psychol 48: 419-426
  20. Jones BT, Corbin WR, Fromme K (2001) A review of expectancy theory and alcohol consumption. Addiction 96: 57-72.
  21. Marlatt GA, Rohsenow DJ (1980) Cognitive processes in alcohol use: Expectancy and the balanced placebo design. In: Mello NK (Ed.), Advances in substance abuse: Behavioral and biological research. Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, 159- 199.
  22. Read JP, Lau-Barraco C, Dunn M, Borsari B (2009) Projected alcohol dose influences on the activation of alcohol expectancies in college drinkers. Alcohol Clin Exp Res33: 1265-1277.
  23. Fromme KD, Amico E (2000) Measuring adolescent alcohol outcome expectancies. Psychol Addict Behav 14: 206-212.
  24. Lee N, Greely J, Oei T (1999) The relationship of positive and negative alcohol expectancies to patterns of consumption of alcohol in social drinker. Addict Behav 24: 359-369.
  25. Palfai T, Wood M (2001) Positive alcohol expectancies and drinking behavior: the influence of expectancy strength and memory accessibility. Psychol Addict Behav15: 60-67.
  26. Fillmore MT, Vogel-Sprott M (1994) Psychomotor performance under alcohol and under caffeine: Expectancy and pharmacological effects. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 2: 319-327.
  27. Huntley ED, Juliano LM (2012) Caffeine expectancy questionnaire (CaffEQ): Construction, psychometric properties, and associations with caffeine use, caffeine dependence, and other related variables. Psychol Assess 24: 592-607.
  28. Ludden AB, Wolfson AR (2010) Understanding Adolescent Caffeine Use: Connecting Use Patterns with Expectancies, Reasons, and Sleep. Health Educ Behav 37: 330-342.
  29. Marlatt GA (1985) Relapse prevention: Theoretical rationale and overview of the model. In: Marlatt GA, Gordon JR (Ed.), Guilford Press, New York.
  30. Fromme K, Stroot E, Kaplin D (1993) Comprehensive Effects of Alcohol: Development and psychometric assessment of a new expectancy questionnaire. Psychological Assessment 5: 19-26.
  31. Fillmore MT, Vogel-Sprott M (1995) Behavioral Effects of Combining Alcohol and Caffeine: Contribution of Drug-Related Expectancies. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol 3: 33-38.
  32. Corbin W, Morean M, Benedict D (2008) The Positive Drinking Consequences Questionnaire (PDCQ): Validation of a new assessment tool. Addict Behav 33: 54-68.
  33. Leigh BC (1989) In search of the seven dwarves: Issues of measurement and meaning in alcohol expectancy research. Psychol Bull 105: 361-373.
  34. Pennay A, Lubman DI (2011) Alcohol and caffeinated energy drinks: A preliminary study exploring patterns of consumption and associated harms. Fitzroy, Melbourne: Turning Point Alcohol and Drug Centre
  35. Park CL (2004) Positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption in college students. Addict Behav 29: 311-321.
  36. Park CL, Grant C (2005) Determinants of positive and negative consequences of alcohol consumption in college students: Alcohol use, gender, and psychological characteristics. Addict Behav 30: 755-765
  37. Miller W, Carroll K (2006) Rethinking substance abuse: What the science shows, and what we should do about it. Guilford Press, New York.
  38. Capron DW, Schmidt NB (2012) Positive drinking consequences among hazardous drinking college students. Addict Behav 37: 663-667.
  39. Lee CM, Patrick ME, Neighbors C, Lewis MA, Tollison SJ, et al. (2010)Exploring the role of positive and negative consequences in understanding perceptions and evaluations of individual drinking events. Addict Behav 35: 764-770.
  40. Logan DE, Henry T, Vaughn M, Luk JW, King KM (2012) Rose-Colored Beer Goggles: The Relation Between Experiencing Alcohol Consequences and Perceived Likelihood and Valence. Psychol Addict Behav 26: 311-317
  41. Patrick ME, Maggs JL (2008) Short-term Changes in Plans to Drink and Importance of Positive and Negative Alcohol Consequences. J Adolesc 31: 307-321.
  42. Stacy AW, Widaman KF, Marlatt GA (1990) Expectancy models of alcohol use. J Pers Soc Psychol 58: 918-928.
  43. Kunin D, Gaskin S, Rogan F, Smith BR, AmitZ (2000) Caffeine promotes ethanol drinking in rats: Examination using a limited-access free choice paradigm. Alcohol 21: 271-277.
  44. Velazquez CE, Poulos NS, Latimer LA, Pasch KE (2012) Association between energy drink consumption and alcohol use behaviors among college students. Drug Alcohol Depend 123: 167-172.
  45. Brache K (2009) Alcohol and cocaine simultaneous polysubstance use: A qualitative investigation. Master’s Thesis, University of Calgary.
  46. Kuntsche E, Stewart S (2009) Why my classmates drink: Drinking motives of classroom peers as predictors of individual drinking motives and alcohol use in adolescence-a meditational model. J Health Psychol 14: 536-546.
  47. Kuntsche E, Knibbe R, Gmel G, Engles R (2006) Replication and validation of the Drinking Motive Questionnaire Revised (DMQ-R, Cooper, 1994) among adolescents in Switzerland. Eur Addict Res 12: 161-168
  48. Nystrom M (1992) Positive and negative consequences of alcohol drinking among young university students in Finland. Br J Addict 87: 715-722.
  49. Arria AM, Caldeira KM, Kasperski SJ, O’Grady KE, Vincent KB, et al. (2010) Increased alcohol consumption, nonmedical prescription drug use, and illicit drug use are associated with energy drink consumption among college students. J Addict Med 4: 74-80.
  50. Miller KE, Quigley BM (2011) Energy drink use and substance use among musicians. J Caffeine Res 1: 67-73.
  51. Miller KE (2008) Wired: energy drinks, jock identity, masculine norms, and risk taking. J Am Coll Health 56: 481-489.
  52. Miller KE (2012) Alcohol Mixed with Energy Drink Use and Sexual Risk-Taking: Casual, Intoxicated, and Unprotected Sex. J Caffeine Res 2: 62-69.
  53. Howland J, Rohsenow DJ, Calise TV, Mackillop J, Metrik J (2011) Caffeinated alcoholic beverages; an emerging public health problem. Am J Prev Med 40: 268-271.
  54. Attwood AS, Rogers PJ, Ataya AF, Adams A, Munafo MR (2012) Effects of caffeine on alcohol-related changes in behavioural control and perceived intoxication in light caffeine consumers. Psychopharmacology 221: 551-560.
  55. Benowitz NL (1990) Clinical pharmacology of caffeine. Annu Rev Med 41: 277-288
  56. Griffiths RR, Woodson PP (1988) Reinforcing effects of caffeine in humans. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 246: 21-29.
  57. Berger LK, Fendrich M, Chen HY, Arria AM, Cisler RA (2011) Sociodemographic correlates of energy drink consumption with and without alcohol: Results of a community survey. Addict Behav 36: 516-519.
  58. Moreira MT, Smith LA, Foxcroft D (2009) Social norms interventions to reduce alcohol misuse in university or college students. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 8: CD006748.
Citation: Divin AL (2013) Perceived Positive Reinforcements Experienced by College Students When Combining Alcohol and Energy Drinks. J Alcoholism Drug Depend 1:110.

Copyright: © 2013 Divin AL. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top