GET THE APP

Development and Validation of an Instrument that Assesses Individ
Journal of Depression and Anxiety

Journal of Depression and Anxiety
Open Access

ISSN: 2167-1044

+44 1223 790975

Research Article - (2018) Volume 7, Issue 3

Development and Validation of an Instrument that Assesses Individual Differences in Threat and Challenge Appraisal

Joe Tomaka1*, Rebecca L. Palacios1, Camilla Champion2 and Stormy Monks3
1Department of Public Health Sciences, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, USA
2Department of Applied Statistics, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, USA
3Department of Emergency Medicine, Texas Tech University Health Sciences Center, Texas, USA
*Corresponding Author: Joe Tomaka, Department of Public Health Sciences, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM 88003575-646-3525, USA, Tel: 575-646-3525 Email:

Keywords: Appraisal; Threat Challenge; Stress; Coping; ACTS Scale; Scale development

Introduction

Cognitive-relational stress theory defines appraisal as the process of classifying situations and events based on their significance for wellbeing [1-3]. In Lazarus and Folkman’s [2] original conception, primary appraisal reflected evaluations of a situation as irrelevant, benignpositive, or stressful whereas secondary appraisal reflected an evaluation of options and resources for coping. “Stressful” primary appraisals included harm/loss, threat, or challenge with harm/loss reflecting events that had already transpired. Threat and challenge, in contrast, were anticipatory reflecting situations lacking a determined outcome. Threat appraisal reflected situations having the perceived potential for harm or loss and produced emotions such as fear, anxiety, and worry. Challenge appraisal reflected situations having the potential for gain, growth, or mastery and produced feelings of eagerness, excitement, and exhilaration [4-8]. Several decades of work on the anticipatory stress appraisals has enhanced the distinction between threat and challenge [5-8]. Such research has further delineated the nature of the appraisal processes in each, identified factors leading to the likelihood of making each type of appraisal, and by identifying the affective, behavioral, and physiological consequences of such appraisals. Regarding delineation of the appraisal process, Tomaka et al. [4] and others [7,8] have suggested that threat and challenge reflect coordinate consideration of both the situation (i.e., primary appraisal) and one’s abilities or resources for coping (i.e., secondary appraisal). As such, threat and challenge reflect different appraisal patterns across primary and secondary appraisal dimensions.

In this view, what makes a situation challenging is the overall perception that although an individual may face significant situational demands, he or she also possesses the resources, skills, or abilities required to deal with such demands. By contrast, what makes a situation threatening is the perception that current demands exceed one’s resources or abilities to cope with them [6,9,10]. This formulation of appraisal as reflecting a relationship between perceived demands and coping abilities is highly consistent with Lazarus and Folkman’s [1] original definition of stress as “a particular relationship between the person and the environment that is appraised by the person as taxing or exceeding his or her resources and endangering his or her wellbeing” (p.19).

To date, numerous studies spanning several decades have supported this conception of threat and challenge appraisals and their respective affective, behavioral, and physiological outcomes. These include initial validation studies [6,11], studies examining personality correlates of threat and challenge [12-17], studies of the implications of threat and challenge for task performance [18-20] and sports performance [21,22], and studies citing implications for health outcomes [23-26].

Individual differences in cognitive appraisals of threat or challenge

Although appraisal is inherently a situational construct, many have recognized that appraisals may also reflect dispositional tendencies [12-16]. Moreover, the notion of stable individual differences in stress appraisal and responding is essential if such responses are to impact health outcomes, particularly those that develop over extended periods of time including coronary heart disease and hypertension [27,28].

Perhaps reflecting these concerns, researchers have developed several measures of cognitive appraisal. Such instruments include semantic differential assessment of potentially stressful appraisals [29]; Peacock and Wong’s Stress Appraisal Measures (SAM) [30] for adults and Stress Appraisal Measure for Adolescents (SAMA) [31]; the Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) [32]; the Stress Appraisal Inventory for Life Situations (SAILS) [33]; and the Cognitive Appraisal Scale (CAS) [34].

Despite the potential utility of such measures, they have share significant limitations. For example, many confound appraisal per se with other aspects of the stress process including aspects of the emotional reaction, overall perceived stressfulness, coping, and personality. Sometimes this confounding is inherent in the assessment technique scale (e.g., Fish’s semantic differentials assessment) and other times it is by the inclusion of items or subscales reflecting constructs other than cognitive appraisal (e.g., Skinner and Brewer’s CAS). As such, these instruments do not measure individual differences in appraisal processes or tendencies per se, but rather assess multiple correlates of threat and challenge experiences including affective and behavioral reactions, stress consequences, coping activities, and stress-related personality dimensions [35,36]. Such confounding is important because, by confounding appraisal (i.e., the initial cognitive processing of a potentially stressful situation) with later stages of the stress response sequence (e.g., affective reactions, behavioral response), such measures will necessarily predict other stress processes such as coping and emotional well-being because of construct confounding, and not because of any true relationship between appraisal processes and adaptational outcomes.

The second limitation of existing measures is that none assesses threat and challenge appraisals in a manner consistent with the psychophysiological studies establishing the construct validity of these constructs. As noted above, considerable research has elucidated threat and challenge as distinct stress processes with unique emotional, behavioral, and health-related outcomes. Such outcomes have included unique physiological patterns with implications for disease [6,7,11,16]; emotional reaction [37], psychological well-being [38], and behavioral performance in sporting situations [22] and in medical emergencies [23]. Despite the utility of the threat and challenge distinction, researchers do not yet have a validated self-report instrument that accurately reflects this body of work on the distinction of threat and challenge appraisals and reactions.

Accordingly, we describe the development and validation of an instrument that assesses individual differences in the tendency to appraise events as threatening or as challenging and that which does so without confounding cognitive appraisal processes with other aspects of stress and coping responses. The first study was exploratory and established the content domains for the Appraisal of Challenge or Threat Scale (ACTS); the second and third studies provided confirmatory evidence of the scale’s factor structure. All studies provided evidence for the construct validity of the scale.

Research Methodology

Participants

Studies 1 and 2 included 166 and 244 college students, respectively. Participants in both were students enrolled in a large University in the southwestern region of the United States. Study 3 included 740 municipal firefighters from a large metropolitan fire department. The university IRB reviewed and approved all studies; students participated voluntarily in exchange for course credit whereas firefighters participated voluntarily as part of their normal training cycle activities. The student samples, from study 1 and study 2, had mean ages (SD) of 23.82 (6.07) and 23.91 (5.88), respectively, were predominantly female (73% and 67%, respectively), and Hispanic (78% and 79%, respectively). The firefighter sample had a mean age of 38.24 (8.06), was predominantly male (98%), and Hispanic (75%).

Measures

Appraisal of Challenge and Threat Scale (ACTS). The Appraisal of Challenge or Threat Scale was based on laboratory research examining threat or challenge appraisals and responses in potentially stressful active coping tasks (i.e., motivated performance situations; Tomaka et al., [6,7,11]. Items ask respondents to appraise a variety of potentially stressful situations that they might face in daily life. The rationale behind the scale was to present respondents with a series of potentially threatening or challenging situations and then to have them appraise that situation in terms of the level of perceived demand and subjective ability to cope. Thus, ACTS “items” reflect common, potentially stressful events that most individuals can at least imagine encountering in daily life and which required or allowed some form of coping response.

Preliminary lists of such events were compiled by approximately 15 research assistants reflecting on their own and others’ experiences. To avoid confounding appraisal with other aspects of the stress and coping response, we eliminated all events initially proposed that described affective and behavioral reactions, coping, and/or stressrelated personality dimensions (e.g., low self-esteem, social anxiety, fear of negative evaluation, and poor overall self-confidence). After the elimination of duplicates, 38 distinct events remained. Sample items included, “You find that someone has said something negative about you,” “You encounter unexpected medical expenses,” “A supervisor asks you to give a speech,” and “You have an argument with a partner or spouse”.

The initial version of the scale presented the 38 items (i.e., events) individually, accompanied by primary and secondary appraisal questions, for primary appraisal, “How demanding is this event to you”, and for secondary appraisal, “How able are you to take action to deal with it?” Each had a 5-point scale ranging from “not at all” to “very much.” The difference between primary and secondary appraisal reflects the degree to which demands exceed resources, or degree of threat (with demands exceeding resources) or challenge (demands countered by resources). Thus, total scores were calculated by first taking the difference between the two appraisal subscales for each event, then taking the mean of the resulting differences to derive overall appraisal tendency scores. Positive scores indicate a tendency to appraise events as threatening, perceiving demand to exceed coping abilities; negative scores indicate a tendency to appraise events as challenging, perceiving demands to be within coping abilities; scores at or near zero reflect no clear tendency. In addition to overall appraisal scores, we also anticipated that the items would cluster into several domains. A priori, we anticipated five domains including self-concerns (health, esteem, identity), financial concerns, work/school hassles, relationships with family and others, and transportation.

Perceived Stress Scale (PSS). The PSS is a 14-item instrument that assesses overall stress levels by measuring the degree to which ongoing situations in life are appraised as stressful [9]. Typical items ask about frequency of experience of stressful emotions and feelings that one was able to keep up with demands. Items include, “how often have you felt nervous and “stressed”, “how often have you felt that things were going your way”, and “how often have you found that you could not cope with all the things that you had to do.” Each item was rated on a frequency scale from 1=never to 5=very often.

Brief Cope. The Brief Cope is a 28-item instrument that assesses individual coping efforts across multiple forms of coping including active coping, planning, positive reframing, acceptance, humor, religion, emotional support, instrumental support, self-distraction, denial venting, substance use, behavioral disengagement, and selfblame [39]. All items asked how the individual has been dealing with stress within the past three months (e.g., “I've been taking action to try to make the situation better”) on scales ranging from 1=I haven’t been doing this at all to 4=I’ve been doing this a lot.

Cognitive Appraisal Scale (CAS). The CAS is an 18-item measure with separate Threat and Challenge subscales [34]. The 10-item Threat Appraisal subscale contains items from existing anxiety and other measures [34,40]. According to the authors, the threat scale assesses the “tendency to focus on possible harm to one’s self-esteem and social identity posed by the disapproval and negative evaluation of others in addition to a low self-confidence in one’s ability to cope with stressful or demanding situations” (p. 681). The 8-item Challenge Appraisal subscale includes one item from another scale [41]. According to the authors [34], “the Challenge Appraisal subscale focused on the anticipation of success and positive outcomes and confidence in one’s capacity to obtain such goals” (p. 681).

PTSD Checklist-Civilian Version (PCL-C). The PCL-C is a 17-item instrument that assesses symptoms of post-traumatic stress within the past 30 days [42]. Items reflect DSM-IV Criteria B, C, and D for PTSD (symptoms of intrusion, avoidance, and hyperarousal). Statements included in the PCL-C involve, “Repeated, disturbing memories, thoughts, or images of stressful experiences from the past?” and “Suddenly acting or feeling as if a stressful experience were happening again (as if you were reliving it)?” Responses are made on a 5-point scales ranging from 1=not at all to 5=extremely.

Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS). The SWLS is a short, 5-item instrument designed to measure global cognitive judgments of one's life [43]. The scale includes items such as, “In most ways, my life is close to my ideal,” “The conditions of my life are excellent,” and “I am satisfied with my life.” Each item is accompanied by a seven-point scale ranging from 1=strongly disagree to 7=strongly agree.

Procedure

For studies 1 and 2, research assistants recruited student volunteers and administered surveys during regularly scheduled class times. Study 1 included a limited number of measures, including the initial iteration of the ACTS; Study 2 included a wider variety of measures. We also assessed basic demographics (e.g., age and gender) in all studies. Study 3 included a census of a metropolitan fire department with approximately 90% of department members completing the survey as part of a larger dissertation study.

Results

Study 1

We first calculated individual appraisal scores at the item level by subtracting the appraisal of coping ability from the appraisals of situational demand and examined each for maintenance of distributional characteristics and potential floor and ceiling effects. All items proved sufficient for inclusion in subsequent analyses. Principal components analyse in SPSS (Version 19.0) examined the initial factor structure of the 38 items. Examination of the resulting eigenvalues, scree plots, and rotated solutions using oblimin rotation suggested that a six-factor model provided the most parsimonious solution. This solution eliminated nine items from the original pool of 38 that did not load 0.32 or greater on any factor.

Table 1 shows the results of this analysis. Factor 1 contained items assessing interpersonal conflict and was labeled Conflict Situations. Factor 2 contained items assessing receipt of medical news, sudden illness, or accidents and was labeled Unexpected Events. Factor 3 contained items relating to giving speeches and making presentations and introducing oneself and was labeled Public Speaking. Factor 4 contained items relating to one’s automobile or commute and was labeled Transportation. Factor 5 contained items relating to meeting new people, being social, and potentially awkward social situations and was labeled Social Anxiety. Finally, Factor 6 contained items assessing credit and banking issues or financial pressures and was labeled Financial.

Variables Component
1 2 3 4 5 6
Conflict Situations
Friends talking behind your back 0.77
Someone said something negative 0.68
Friend or family says something bad 0.66
Argument with spouse/partner 0.61
Said something you later regretted 0.52
Coworker complained to boss about you 0.39
Unexpected Events
Receive unwanted medical news -0.8
Close relative has a heart attack -0.78
Find out you have a chronic disease -0.77
Involved in a car accident -0.59
Unexpected medical expenses -0.56
Public Speaking
Supervisor asks you to give a speech 0.82
Asked to give major presentation 0.78
Asked to introduce yourself in public 0.77
Going on a job interview 0.51
Receive a major work/school assignment 0.47
Transportation
Lost your keys -0.82
Discover a flat tire -0.72
Car breaks down at rush hour -0.34 -0.67
Lock your keys in the car -0.66
Car won't start before work - -0.53 0.35 -
Social Anxiety
Forcing self to meet new people 0.69
Go to party where don’t know anyone 0.68
Not enough money at grocery store 0.43 -0.33
Going on a blind date 0.31 -0.38
Financial Issues
Credit card turned down at store -0.77
Notice of bank overdraft fee -0.66
Credit card increases minimum payment -0.49
Having to juggle financial pressures  -  0.41 -0.44

Note:  loadings <0.30 have been

Table 1: Factor loadings for principal components analysis with six factor model with oblimin rotation.

As shown across the top of Table 2, subscales resulting from these factors all showed acceptable levels of internal consistency with coefficient alphas ranging from 0.74 to 0.85; the overall score across the 29 items was also internally consistent (α=0.92). Table 2 also shows correlations between the ACTS dimensions and total score, and other demographic and stress-related outcomes. The ACTS correlated strongly, but not redundantly, with the PSS. In general, the ACTS overall score and dimensions positively correlated with stress and use of various avoidance coping strategies. Finally, Table 2 also shows the intercorrelations among the ACTS subscales and total score. Like associations with the PSS, the subscale intercorrelations were strong but non-redundant, suggesting the discriminant validity of the six dimensions.

Variables Total Appraisal Conflict Situations Unexpected Events Public Speaking Transport Social Anxiety Financial Concerns
(α=0.92) (α=0.83) (α=.85) (α=0.83) (α=0.84) (α=0.74) (α=0.74)
Age -0.09 -0.09 -0.21** 0.07 -0.03 -0.07 -0.05
Gender (1=M; 2=F) 0.24** 0.20** 0.20* 0.16* 0.23** 0.20** 0.05
Perceived Stress Scale 0.51** 0.46** 0.33** 0.35** 0.39** 0.34** 0.42**
Factor 1
Approach Coping -0.06 0.01 - -0.08 -0.07 -0.15 -0.01
Positive Reinterpretation -0.13 -0.15 0.01 -0.16* -0.03 -0.23** -0.05
Active Coping -0.06 -0.05 -0.03 -0.03 -0.02 -0.09 -0.06
Planning -0.14 -0.09 -0.09 -0.07 -0.08 -.19* -0.14
Inst. Social Support 0.03 0.13 0.04 0 -0.03 -0.12 0.07
Suppression of Activities 0.04 0.06 0.02 0.07 -0.03 -0.02 0.11
Emotional Social Support 0.09 .21** -0.04 0.04 0.05 0.01 0.12
Acceptance -0.13 -0.14 0.02 -0.11 -0.12 -.21** -0.04
Restraint 0.01 0.02 0.03 -0.02 0.04 -0.03 -
Religious Coping 0.07 0.1 0.08 -0.02 0.04 0.06 0.01
Humor -0.17* -0.13 -0.04 -0.14 -0.23** -0.14 -0.11
Factor 2
Avoidance Coping 0.34** 0.39** 0.18* 0.26* 0.19* 0.23** 0.25**
Denial 0.23** 0.28** 0.12 0.20* 0.08 0.14 0.17*
Behavioral Disengage 0.29** 0.32** 0.09 0.27** 0.16* 0.23** 0.22**
Mental Disengagement 0.19* 0.17* 0.19* 0.15* 0.12 0.09 0.11
Substance Use 0.11 0.13 0.04 0.06 0.04 0.07 0.17*
Focus On/Vent Emotions 0.33** 0.41** 0.18* 0.19* 0.25** 0.24** 0.16*
Total Appraisal 0.85** 0.72** 0.68** 0.78** 0.73** 0.77**
Conflict Situations 0.49** 0.52** 0.57** 0.57** 0.59**
Unexpected Events 0.32** 0.56** 0.35** 0.51**
Public Speaking 0.38** 0.48** 0.47**
Transport 0.47** 0.50**
Social Anxiety 0.50**

N=166; *p<0.05, **p<0.01.

Table 2: Study 1: Correlations between appraisal scales, stress, and coping (College students).

Study 2 and Study 3

Based on Study 1 findings, we modified the scale slightly for administration to samples 2 and 3. Most notably, and in order to keep the scale to a manageable number of items, we retained only the four best-loading items for each factor. This resulted in a scale that retained the six dimensions described above but with only four items per subscale totaling in 24 items. We administered this version to the second sample of college students and a census of municipal firefighters. Both studies also included additional measures (described above) to assess the convergent and discriminant validity of the ACTS.

Confirmatory factor analyses

We used SPSS AMOS (19.0) to conduct confirmatory factor analyses on the data from Studies 2 and 3. The specific model tested had four items loading on six corresponding factors. The model also specified correlations (i.e., covariances) among the six factors. Table 3 and Figure 1 summarize the results of these analyses. As shown, the model showed reasonably good fit across the two independent samples. Loadings for the items on their respective factors were all relatively high (>0.55) and consistent across samples.

depression-anxiety-factory-analyses

Figure 1: Confirmatory factory analyses of the ACTS in two samples, one with college students and one with municipal firefighters.

Model CMIN/DF NFI CFI RMSEA
Six Correlated Factors (Baseline Model)
Study 2 588/255 2.3 0.9 0.94 0.073
Study 3 753/209 3.6 0.93 0.95 0.059
Single Latent Construct/One Factor Model
Study 2 1006/245 4.11 0.73 0.77 0.113
Study 3 2096/224 9.35 0.79 0.81 0.106
Hierarchical Latent Construct Model
Study 2 648/264 2.46 0.89 0.93 0.077
Study 3 876/218 4.02 0.91 0.93 0.064

n=244; n=740 

Table 3: Fit indices for alternative models derived from study 2 data. 

Table 3 also shows fit indices for the hypothesized and two alternative models including a single factor model and a hierarchical model. The single factor model specified all items loading on a single latent construct (i.e., “appraisal”), whereas the hierarchical model retained the six dimensions, but also specified a second order latent construct (i.e., “appraisal”) instead of specifying covariances among them. As shown, the goodness of fit indices were relatively poor for the single latent construct model, especially compared with the six correlated factors model. Fit indices for the hierarchical latent construct model were better but still worse than, the six correlated factors model. Overall, the six correlated factors model provides the best fit to the data across the two diverse samples.

Construct validity student and fire-fighter samples

Table 4 shows scale reliabilities and correlations between ACTS total and dimensional scores and various demographic and stress-andcoping- related measures among our second sample of students. This analysis included only two primary coping dimensions from the COPE, identified using principal components analyses. ACTS subscales again showed acceptable levels of internal consistency with coefficient alphas ranging from 0.77 to 0.88 for each of the four-item subscales and the overall score based on 24 items reaching 0.94. Consistent positive correlations with gender again suggested that women appraised the events as more threatening (i.e., less challenging) than did men. The ACTS related consistently and positively to the experience of stress as assessed by the PSS. The ACTS was also associated with reports of the greater use of both approach and avoidance-focused coping, although associations with avoidance coping were consistently larger. As expected, the ACTS negatively related to the experience of challenge, as assessed by the CAS Challenge subscale, and positively related to the experience of threat, as assessed by the CAS Threat subscale. The ACTS related consistently to measures of well-being, including positive associations with reports of PTSD symptoms and correspondingly negative associations with life satisfaction. Finally, Table 4 shows the intercorrelations among the ACTS subscales and total score. The subscale intercorrelations again suggest considerable, but non-redundant, overlap among the six dimensions.

 Variables Total Appraisal (α=.94) Conflict Situations (α=.81) Unexpected Events (α=.85) Public Speaking (α=.86) Transport (α=.88) Social Anxiety (α=.77) Financial Concerns (α=.81)
Gender 0.25* 0.21* 0.30* 0.11* 0.28* 0.13* 0.12*
Income -0.06 -0.06 0.03 -0.12 -0.07 -0.05 -0.02
PSS Stress 0.47* 0.40* 0.36* 0.34* 0.34* 0.32* 0.48*
Avoidance Coping 0.30* 0.30* 0.14* 0.21* 0.21* 0.26* 0.32*
Approach Coping 0.17* 0.19* 0.21* 0.03 0.20* 0.03 0.15*
CAS Challenge -0.31* -0.27* -0.17* -0.25* -0.24* -0.28* -0.29*
CAS Threat 0.41* 0.42* 0.22* 0.35* 0.27* 0.38* 0.34*
PTSD Symptoms 0.28* 0.24* 0.20* 0.24* 0.21* 0.21* 0.24*
Life Satisfaction -0.23* -0.26* -0.11* -0.18* -0.13* -0.20* -0.22*
Total Appraisal 0.84* 0.78* 0.73* 0.85* 0.75* 0.80*
Conflict Situations 0.61* 0.48* 0.69* 0.58* 0.62*
Unexpected Events 0.37* 0.72* 0.44* 0.56*
Public Speaking 0.48* 0.57* 0.52*
Transport 0.50* 0.63*
Social Anxiety - - - - - 0.49*

Note:  *Correlation is significant at p<0.05; PSS=Perceived Stress Scale, CAS=Cognitive Appraisal Scale 

Table 4: Study 2: Correlations between ACTS dimensions and other variables and intercorrelations among ACTS subscales among college students.

Table 5 shows the results of similar analyses conducted on the municipal firefighter sample. Here, the principal components analysis of the cope subscales revealed an additional factor, Support Coping, which consisted of instrumental and emotional support joined with religion. As shown across the top of Table 5, the subscales again showed acceptable levels of internal consistency with coefficient alphas ranging from 0.72 to 0.88 for each of the four-item subscales and the 24-item total scale score reaching 0.93.

 Variables Total Appraisal (α=0.93) Conflict Situations (α=0.82) Unexpected Events (α=0.85) Public Speaking (α=0.86) Transport (α=88) Social Anxiety (α=72) Financial concerns (α=0.81)
Age -0.09* -0.08* -0.12** -0.02 -0.07* -0.03 -0.12**
Perceived Stress Scale 0.47** 0.41** 0.35** 0.29** 0.39** 0.40** 0.44**
Approach Coping 0.16** 0.15** 0.16** 0.05 0.13** 0.13** 0.17**
Avoidance Coping 0.32** 0.30** 0.23** 0.19** 0.29** 0.28** 0.27**
Support Coping 0.10** 0.12** 0.11** 0.01 0.10** 0.06 0.06
Total Appraisal  - 0.85** 0.80** 0.74** 0.85** 0.80** 0.81**
Conflict Situations  -  - 0.63** 0.49** 0.69** 0.65** 0.64**
Unexpected Events  -  - - 0.50** 0.65** 0.50** 0.65**
Public Speaking -  - - - 0.52** 0.60** 0.44**
Transport  - - - 0.62** 0.67**
Social Anxiety -  - -  - - - 0.55**

 N=704; *p<0.05, **p<0.01 

Table 5: Study 3. Correlations between ACTS dimensions and other variables and intercorrelations among ACTS subscales (Municipal fire fighters).

The ACTS inversely related with age, with younger firefighters reporting greater propensity to make threat appraisals than older firefighters. The ACTS related positively to the experience of stress as assessed by the PSS. The ACTS was again positively associated with report of coping, both approach and avoidance-focused. Again, associations with avoidance coping were more consistently larger. Correlations of threat appraisal with support coping were small but generally positive. Finally, Table 5 shows the intercorrelations among the subscales and total score. The subscale intercorrelations again suggest considerable, but non-redundant, overlap among the six dimensions.

Independent prediction of stress-related outcomes

One indicator of a new measure’s utility is its ability to predict variance in outcomes above that predicted by existing measures. To assess this, we examined how the ACTS related to symptoms associated with depression and post-traumatic stress disorder independent of stress as assessed by the PSS [9]. As described above, zero-order analyses showed that both the ACTS and the PSS related to these outcomes at the univariate level (r’s ranging between 0.38 and 0.64, all p<0.001). Hierarchical multiple regression analyses controlling for PSS levels indicated that the ACTS predicted variance in depression (R-Square Change=0.02; b=0.16, p<0.001) and post-traumatic stress (R-Square change=0.03, b=0.19, p<0.001) over and above that predicted by the PSS. The results suggest that cognitive appraisal tendencies that favor the appraisal of life events as threatening (i.e., perceived demands in excess of coping abilities) assess a unique component of these potentially clinical conditions.

Discussion

Overall, the results of the three studies described above provided consistent evidence for the reliability and validity of the Appraisal of Challenge and Threat Scale (ACTS) as a measure of individual differences in the tendency to make threat or challenge appraisals overall and within several life domains. Three independent samples, two of college students and the third of municipal firefighters, showed the instrument to be highly reliable and to have a consistent factor structure assessing threat or challenge. Intercorrelations among the subscales suggested consistency, but non-redundancy among the domain measures.

The three studies also provided considerable evidence for the construct validity of the ACTS. Specifically, the ACTS correlated sufficiently and non-redundantly with other scales measuring perceived stress [9], the experience of threat and challenge [34,40], and coping [39]. The ACTS also correlated expectedly with stress-related outcomes including PTSD symptoms and life satisfaction. Finally, hierarchical regression analyses showed the ability of the ACTS to assess unique variance in stress-related outcomes.

One of the most consistent findings was that higher ACTS scores (indicative of threat appraisal) were associated with greater use of avoidant forms of coping—whether assessed as individual coping strategies as in Study 1 or as composite variables (Studies 2 and 3). Collectively, avoidant forms of coping include such things as substance use coping, behavioral disengagement, and mental disengagement. ACTS scores also correlated with approach forms coping. Specifically, although Study 1 showed only weak associations between ACTS scores and individual approach-related coping dimensions, Studies 2 and 3 provided more consistent evidence using a composite and more reliable indicator of approach coping.

The associations between ACTS scores and coping activities are noteworthy for two reasons. First, they are consistent with laboratory analogs of coping which consistently showed threat appraisal (relative to challenge) to be associated with poor behavioral coping (i.e., task performance) [6,11,16,44]. The laboratory studies speculated that challenge appraisal led to problem-focused coping, whereas, threat appraisal led to emotion-focused coping. Viewed this way, the pattern of correlations between the ACTS and avoidance coping (i.e., emotionfocused coping) support the laboratory study contentions.

Second, the approach coping results may tell something unique regarding how threat and challenge appraisal tendencies relate to realworld coping. Specifically, the positive associations of threat appraisal with approach (and avoidance) coping suggest more coping in threat appraisal rather than qualitatively different coping. In other words, threat appraisal may lead not just to a preference for emotion-focused coping as suggested above, but the need to engage multiple coping efforts, both emotion- and problem-focused. Factors such as poorer execution of initial coping efforts, or failure to choose coping strategies that meet the demands of the situation, and hence the subsequent need to apply more strategies, may underlie these effects. In contrast, challenge appraisal may be associated with less coping because of greater initial success and/or more judicious matching of coping efforts to situational demands.

Future studies might address a number of issues. The first is the temporal stability of the instrument. Despite high internal consistency, the present studies did not address the temporal stability of ACTS scores. Overall, one might expect the ACTS to have temporal stability in proportion to other stress-related constructs. Stress varies with time and experience and ACTS scores might reflect such variability. It is also possible that ACTS scores would show considerable stability over time and situations, reflecting more stable aspects of personality.

Reducing appraisal of challenge or threat to a unidimensional scale can obfuscate some results related to both primary and secondary appraisals. However, unidimensional reduction is a common approach in statistical applications. Additionally, this approach is consistent with definitions of challenge and threat in laboratory experiments. Dimensionality reduction can allow for a more thorough analysis of challenge/threat appraisal while simplifying results, providing a better understanding of overall outcomes [45]. Further analyses may determine the relative importance of primary and secondary appraisals for those who are interested.

Another issue for future research might be the developmental antecedents of the tendency toward threat or challenge appraisal. For example, how do early experiences with stressful situations shape later appraisal tendencies? Indeed, Dienstbier’s [46] model of physiological toughness suggests that success experiences with controllable stressors lead to challenge appraisals and physiological toughness. In contrast, uncontrollable stress and failure to cope lead to threat and learned helplessness. Do similar processes shape the development of individual differences in threat and challenge appraisal as assessed by the ACTS?

Future research should also address how ACTS scores relate to appraisals and stress experiences in acute laboratory situations. Presumably, ACTS scores should predict situational appraisals and responses in a manner consistent with those seen in laboratory studies of threat and challenge, the phenomenon that led to the development of the ACTS in the first place.

Finally, future studies should examine whether and how ACTS scores predict short- and long-term health outcomes, including studies of the potential mechanisms of disease. Here a study linking ACTS scores to biological mechanisms such as corticosteroids and cytokines would be particularly pertinent. Parallel studies might address psychological outcomes, including stress disorder, depression, or life satisfaction.

Conclusion

In summary, the ACTS assesses individual differences in cognitive appraisal of threat and challenge using items that are not confounded with other stress-related constructs, including affective reactions, behavioral reactions, and stress consequences, coping, and personality. The ACTS should enhance research in not only the fields of stress, emotion, and coping, but also social, personality, and health psychology; and clinical research areas including trauma, depression and emotional disorders. Overall, we hope that this instrument opens up new avenues for research on these important stress-related constructs.

References

  1. Lazarus RS (1966) Psychological stress and the cognitive process. New York: McGraw-Hill, USA.
  2. Lazarus RS, Folkman S (1984) Stress, appraisal, and coping. Springer, New York, USA.
  3. Lazarus RS (1991) Emotion and adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press, USA.
  4. Blascovich J, Mendes WB, Tomaka J, Salomon K, Seery MD (2003) The robust nature of the biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat: A reply to Wright and Kirby. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 7: 234-243.
  5. Blascovich J (2013) The biopsychosocial model of challenge and threat: Reflections, theoretical ubiquity, and new directions. Neuroscience of prejudice and intergroup relations Psychology Press 229-243.
  6. Tomaka J, Blascovich J, Kelsey RM, Leitten CL (1993) Subjective, physiological, and behavioral effects of threat and challenge appraisal. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 18: 616-624.
  7. Blascovich J, Tomaka J (1996) The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation. In M. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology. New York: Academic Press 28: 1-51.
  8. Seery MD (2011) Challenge or threat? Cardiovascular indexes of resilience and vulnerability to potential stress in humans. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35: 1603-1610.
  9. Cohen S, Kamarck T, Mermelstein R (1983) A global measure of perceived stress. J Health Soc Behav 24: 385-396.
  10. Weisbuch M, Seery MD, Ambady N, Blascovich J (2009) On the correspondence between physiological and nonverbal responses: Nonverbal behavior accompanying challenge and threat. J Nonverbal Behav 33: 141-148.
  11. Tomaka J, Blascovich J, Kibler J, Ernst JM (1997) Cognitive and physiological antecedents of threat and challenge appraisal. J Pers Soc Psychol 73: 63-72.
  12. Penley JA, Tomaka J (2002) The association of coping to physical and psychological health outcomes: A meta-analytic review. J Behav Med 25: 551-603.
  13. Schneider TR (2004) The role of neuroticism on psychological and physiological stress responses. ‎J Exp Soc Psychol 40: 795-804.
  14. Schneider TR, Rench TA, Lyons JB, Riffle RR (2011) The influence of neuroticism, extraversion, and openness on stress responses. Stress and Health 28: 102-110.
  15. Tomaka J, Blascovich J (1994) Effects of justice beliefs on cognitive appraisal of, and subjective, physiological, and behavioral responsese to, potential stress. J Pers Soc Psychol 67: 732-740.
  16. Tomaka J, Palacios R, Schneider KT, Colotla M, Concha JB, et al. (1999) Assertiveness predicts threat and challenge reactions to potential stress among women. J Pers Soc Psychol 76: 1008-1021.
  17. Seery MD, Weisbuch M, Blascovich J (2009) Something to gain, something to lose: The cardiovascular consequences of outcome framing. Int J Psychophysiol 73: 308-312.
  18. Gildea KM, Shenider T, Shebilske WL (2007) Stress appraisals and training performance on a complex laboratory task. Hum Factors 49: 745-758.
  19. Jamieson JP, Mendes WB, Blackstock E, Schmader T (2010) Turning the knots in your stomach into bows: Reappraising arousal improves performance on the GRE. J Exp Soc Psychol 46: 208-212.
  20. Ohly S, Fritz C (2010) Work characteristics, challenge appraisal, creativity, and proactive behavior: A multi-level study. J Organ Behav 31: 543-565.
  21. Blascovich J, Seery MD, Mugridge CA, Norris RK, Weisbuch M (2004) Predicting athletic performance from cardiovascular indexes of challenge and threat. J Exp Soc Psychol 40: 683-688.
  22. Hase A, O’Brien J, Moore LJ, Freeman P (2018) The relationship between challenge and threat states and performance: A systematic review. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology. Advance online publication.
  23. Harvey A, Nathens AB, Bandiera G, LeBlanc VR (2010) Threat and challenge: Cognitive appraisal and stress responses in simulated trauma resuscitations J Med Educ 44: 587-594.
  24. Mendes WB, Blascovich J, Hunter SB, Lickel B, Jost JT (2007) Threatened by the unexpected: Physiological responses during social interactions with expectancy-violating partners. J Pers Soc Psychol 92: 698-716.
  25. O’Donovan A, Tomiyama AJ, Lin J, Puterman E, Adler NE, et al. (2012) Stress Appraisals and biological aging: A key role for anticipatory threat in the relationship between psychological stress and telomere length. Brain Behav Immun 26: 573-579.
  26. Schneider KT, Tomaka J, Palacios R (2001) Women’s cognitive, affective, and physiological reactions to a male coworker’s sexist behavior. J Appl Soc Psychol 31: 1995-2018.
  27. Steptoe A (1991) The links between stress and illness. J Psychosom Res 35: 633-644.
  28. Lovallo WR, Gerin W (2003) Psychophysiological reactivity: Mechanisms and pathways to cardiovascular disease. Psychosom Med 65: 36-45.
  29. Fish TA (1986) Semantic differential assessment of benign, threat and challenge appraisals of life events. Can J Behav Sci 18:1-13.
  30. Peacock EJ, Wong PTP (1990) The stress appraisal measure (SAM): A multidimensional approach to cognitive appraisal. Stress Med 6: 227-236.
  31. Rowley AA, Roesch SC, Jurica BJ, Vaughn AA (2005) Developing and validating a stress appraisal measure for minority adolescents. J. Adolesc 28: 547-557.
  32. Ferguson E, Matthews G, Cox T (1999) The Appraisal of Life Events (ALE) scale: Reliability and validity. Br J Health Psychol 4: 97-116.
  33. Groomes DA, Leahy MJ (2002) The relationships among the stress appraisal process, coping disposition, and level of acceptance of disability. Rehabilitation Counselling Bulletin 46: 14-23.
  34. Skinner N, Brewer N (1999) The dynamics of threat and challenge appraisals prior to stressful achievement events. J Pers Soc Psychol 83: 678-692.
  35. Morales-Monks S, Tomaka J, Coltrane A (2011) The appraisal of stressful events scale. Ann Behav Med 42: S44.
  36. Tomaka J, Morales-Monks S (2013) Threat and challenge appraisals predict behavioral and psychological health outcomes. Ann Behav Med 44: SXXX.
  37. Herrald MM, Tomaka J (2002) Patterns of emotion-specific appraisal, coping, and cardiovascular reactivity during an ongoing emotional episode. J Pers Soc Psychol 83: 434-450.
  38. Blascovich J (2008) Challenge, threat, and health. Handbook of Motivation Science NY, Guilford Press, USA.
  39. Carver CS (1997) You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: Consider the Brief COPE. Int J Behav Med 4: 92-100.
  40. Skinner N, Brewer N (2002) The dynamics of threat and challenge appraisals prior to stressful achievement events. J Pers Soc Psychol 83: 678-692.
  41. Norem JK, Cantor N (1986) Anticipatory and post hoc cushioning strategies: Optimism and defensive pessimism in “risky” situations. Cognit Ther Res 10: 347-362.
  42. Ruggiero KJ, Ben KD, Scotti JR, Rabalais AE (2003) Psychometric properties of the PTSD Checklist-Civilian Version. J Trauma Stres 16: 495-502.
  43. Pavot W, Diener E (1993) Reviews of the Satisfaction with Life Scale. Psychol Assess 5: 164-172.
  44. Tomaka J, Palacios-Esquivel RL (1997) Motivational systems and stress-related cardiovascular reactivity. Motiv Emot 21: 275-296.
  45. Brenninkmeijer V, Van Yperen N (2003) How to conduct research on burnout: Advantages and disadvantages of a unidimensional approach in burnout research. Occupational and environmental medicine 60: i16-i20.
  46. Dienstbier RA (1989) Arousal and physiological toughness: Implications for mental and physical health. Psychol Rev 96: 84-100.
Citation: Tomaka J, Palacios RL, Champion C, Monks S (2018) Development and Validation of an Instrument that Assesses Individual Differences in Threat and Challenge Appraisal. J Depress Anxiety 7: 313.

Copyright: © 2018 Tomaka J, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.
Top